Printer-friendly format  Article citation        



 

Drid Williams: The Australian Papers

 
 

Survey of Australian Literature on Aboriginal Dancing

Drid Williams

But whether one deplores or rejoices in the fact, there are still zones in which savage (that is, untamed) thought, like savage species, is relatively protected. This is the case of art, to which our civilization accords the status of a national park, with all the advantages and inconveniences attending so artificial a formula; and it is particularly the case of so many as yet 'uncleared' sectors of social life, where, through indifference or inability, and most often without our knowledge why, primitive thought continues to flourish.

Claude Levi-Strauss, La Pensée Sauvage, 219

Introduction

To my mind rightly so, Stanner has said, "Contemporary study (of religion, totemism and symbolism) is weakened by the fact that there is so much bias in the old printed record. One cannot turn very hopefully to it for test or confirmation of new insights. Far too much of the information was the product of minds caught up with special pleadings of one kind or another" (1979: 123). Exactly the same things could be said about studies of the dance (or dances or dancing) in Australia. With regard to the dancing of Aboriginal peoples in particular, the comment above is relevant because a large proportion, although not all, of the dancing referred to in the printed record pertains to religion, totemism, or symbolism.

     Following Stanner, I would want to ask about studies of dancing, Aboriginal or otherwise, "What are the present limits of our information and what would constitute a reasonable estimate of the present theoretical position(s) concerning the subject of dancing on this continent?" Secondarily, one would want to ask how the answers to these questions would compare with answers which might be given about, say, American dancing, British dancing, or any other nation's store of information about the dances and dancers included in its politically defined borders.

     Conclusions formed on the basis of intensive study of written sources upon arrival in Australia on August 5, 1986, are these: a definitive survey of the state of Australian dancing awaits the possibility of more profound assessment (1) after the separation of the real problems of scholarly dance studies become separated from false, mis-stated, or imaginary problems, especially with regard to theoretical and methodological questions, and (2) development of a level of thinking and scholarship about the dance that is at present comparatively rare, mainly because of the emphasis at a technical school and college level on the performance of dancing, that is, the training of dancers. On the whole, this emphasis exists at that level of education in all English-speaking countries, but in Australia, there are no possibilities at this writing for dancers, teachers, choreographers, or directors of companies to pursue further education regarding their subject at university (see Appendix A for information from a recent government survey).

     Moreover, the notion of 'performance' in the tertiary level of education which is available to persons interested in the dance is limited to, indeed dominated by, the study of ballet or various forms of Western contemporary dancing (for example, Martha Graham technique, Cunningham, Horton, and others). Writing about dances or thinking about them (and the epistemological and ontological issues that such writing and thinking entail) is rare, as is the comparative study of danced forms, their analysis, and/or their relations to other movement-based phenomena, although there are some anthropological works of excellent caliber: Kaeppler on Tonga, Schieffelin on New Guinea, von Sturmer on Cape York dancing, Wild and Clunies-Ross on Walpiri and Arnhem peoples, Goodale on Tiwi dancing—but they are too few.

     For some of the reasons why this situation prevails, we can again turn to Stanner, whose critical comments about those who influenced the study of religion in the past can be brought equally to bear upon studies of dancing (see Stanner 1979: 106–43 with reference to Durkheim, Frazer, Lang, Strehlow, Freud, and others). One would only want to add some of the names of those who have dealt specifically not with Aboriginal dancing, but with the notion of the dance on a global scale: Lilly Grove (Lady Frazer) who wrote in 1895, Harrison (1913), Havemeyer (1916), Ridgeway (1915), Havelock Ellis (1920), and, later on, Hambly (1926), Sachs (1937), Kurath (1960), Lange (1975), and others too numerous to mention here (see Keali'inohomoku 1980 [1969] and Williams 2004 [1991] for further discussion and citations).

     The undertaking of a definitive survey of literature about dancing specifically in Australia and with reference to her social context cannot be made manifest in depth until there exists a cadre of Australian scholars of Aboriginal, English, or other ethnic Australian origins with post-graduate university degrees who will be in a position to contribute authoritative theoretical, historical, and ethnographic analyses of the subject. These are simply not available at present since the state of the art in 1987–88 does not include such scholars. Specifically, I refer to those who, besides anthropological and/or ethnomusicological and linguistic training, have adequate knowledge and experience in structured systems of human movement, including the dance. These scholars would also have to possess requisite literacy skills with regard to human action sign systems.

     Of the studies of dancing that presently exist, there are none of which I am aware that emphasizes comparative materials or commentary among the many forms of dancing of Aboriginal peoples or between those forms of dancing, signing, or rituals and those of the rest of multicultural Australia. One would wish to stress that there are no overtones of reproach intended here. The same could be said, with a few notable exceptions,1 of nearly all American and British studies of dancing as well. Australia does not lag very far behind any other English-speaking country in this way, but the lag which does exist is there mainly because her dancers and composers of dancing do not possess the means to develop scholarship in the subject, except by competing for grants in overseas institutions. Owing to the efforts of a few concerned persons,2 this country could be in a position in the future to assume leadership in this field, possibly providing adequate models for other countries with regard to the direction that dance and human movement studies might usefully take at university-level educational institutions.

     The following survey of specific studies of dances are those which anyone would have to cope with in an attempt to discover the limits, potentialities, and substance of research in the subject of dancing up to now in Australia. The studies represented have been chosen from a recent bibliographical overview of writings on Aboriginal dances and dancing (see Appendix B which is a reproduction of Wild 1986).3

     Before undertaking the survey, however, it seems important to raise a point which Stanner made in a different context that is highly relevant, especially to the items from the bibliography which are anthropologically uninformed:

One of the troubles of course is that concepts like 'society' and 'culture' are being used without technical understanding. One often has the strong impression that 'culture' is used to mean only mythology, bark-painting and dancing. I read recently that one politician would reduce it to bark-painting only (1979: 313).

     Then, too, we must bear in mind the fact that anthropology has not had much to say about Western forms of dancing until recently;4 but then, until recently, it has not had much to say about Western forms of anything. It is perhaps understandable that for a long time in the relatively short history of the discipline, anthropology was about 'them,' not 'us,' although this has changed drastically over the past thirty years (see Crick 1975, for the shift in British anthropology from "function" to "meaning;" Burridge (1973: 43–84), for further clarification of the western notion of Aborigines as "other" and as "primitive;" and a recent ASA monograph, Anthropology at Home (Jackson 1987), which treats some of the developments among British-trained anthropologists since the mid-1970s).

     Further to the point, a great deal is laid at anthropology's doors by dance scholars who, wishing for something novel and often with a view toward marketable goods in the literary field, have turned to 'ethnology' as a source of subject matter and inspiration. The history of thought about dancing in social and cultural anthropology itself, by contrast, bears some striking resemblances to the history of thought about 'primitive religion' (see Williams 1976a, for a criticism of my own pre-anthropological writings). In our own culture, the dance has been regularly stigmatized, applauded, derided, and elevated over the years in much the same way as drama and dramatic acting (see Barish 1981, for further discussion).

     The point of all this? It is a mistake to imagine that serious study of the subject of dancing is somehow meant only to preserve the 'Culture'5 and/or memories of an 'ethnic,' a white Australian or an Aboriginal past, or that the study of dancing in any of its manifestations is the kind of feckless enterprise that public opinion often imagines it to be. Dancers as a group (a sub-culture, perhaps?) in English-speaking societies are in some sense a 'marginal' group, no matter what form of dancing they practice; they are often classified with 'primitives' (see Williams 1988, for further discussion) and indeed, by the middle of the 1950s in the United States, 'primitive dance' had come to refer to

a kind of pseudo Afro-Caribbean type of dance. This so-called primitive dance has been stylized . . . until it has become a kind of contrived tradition in itself. But are Afro-Caribbeans primitive? The answer is that these groups whether or not we can designate them as 'primitive' have their own dance traditions which are totally unlike each other. There is no such thing as 'primitive dance.' The term is meaningless. (Keali'inohomoku 1970: 90)

     Dancers throughout the world are, on the whole, familiar with stereotypes, fantasies, and wrong-headed theories about themselves and their activities because they often have to contend with them on a daily basis. If it is the case that Aborigines in Australia are still labeled "primitive," for example, then it is equally true that, in much popular thinking, dancers are labeled in the same or similar ways. They seem to be regarded as the 'primitives' of the Western art world.

     There are immense tasks of scholarship awaiting attention in the field of dancing and human movement study. In general, the literature on the subject in the English language is extremely poor. It is important, therefore, that these introductory remarks are understood in the spirit in which they are intended: as an overall critique which is meant to point to the need for a different kind of scholarship with reference to Aboriginal and every other form of dancing in Australia. To comprehend more specifically what is meant, I refer to scholarship that is not based on the kinds of cosmetic treatments of the subject that are all too familiar to those of us who have read it all, from even cursory examination of the titles and subject matters of many of the dance books to be found in performing arts sections of the majority of bookshops. I also refer to a genre of professional writing which is not cosmetic but is equally disappointing for other reasons, for example, Spencer (1985) and Richard Moyle's contribution to a recently published festschrift for Alice Moyle (1984). It is to an article in an edited collection by Berndt and Phillips (1973) that we will now turn.

Australian Aboriginal Dance (Allen 1973)

Apart from Allen's article, an MFA thesis appeared in this year, namely, Quisenberry (1973). Allen's article is better than Quisenberry's thesis, partly because, unlike the thesis, Allen's work is neither sentimental nor patronizing, nor does it depend, as Quisenberry's work does, for its inspiration dance-wise on the outmoded and basically misguided theories of Curt Sachs about dancing (1937: 103–4).6 The dependence on Sachs is a common flaw of many American dance-education theses on the subject. Anthropologically, it bespeaks a lack of critical discrimination regarding historical and theoretical sources.

     Allen's contribution is not crippled by the same kind of thing as Quisenberry's thesis; however, I use her work as a stalking horse to begin this survey because (1) consideration of the kind of work she did raises several important issues with regard to written accounts of dancing; (2) comparison of this kind of dance study with other possibilities of approach can be instructive; and (3) the author is a trained choreologist and dancer whose work is uninformed by anthropological, linguistic, or philosophical insights into the study of dancing.

     Allen possesses a finely tuned appreciation of the movement aspects of dancing, and she is an experienced notator. She brings considerable intuitive understanding to her study, for example, of the need for separating the constituent (called "important") features of the movement patterns from the contingent (called "incidental") features of the patterns under investigation, but these insights are spoiled when she remarks that "Aboriginal dancing is based to a large extent on spontaneity which means that nothing is done exactly the same way a second time" (Allen 1973: 276).

     The difficulties with her work are fourfold:

  1. Innocent of scholarly discourse on the problems implied in the phrase "nothing is done exactly the same way a second time," the author falls into a well-known philosophical trap. That is, if it is the case that nothing is done the same way a second time, then how does she know it is the same dance? Or, given that she means simply that there is a conventionally accepted latitude of performing 'steps' and 'figures' in the dance (a parallel issue in spoken language would involve, for example, how we recognize the meaning of b/a/th or b/ae/th when confronted with transatlantic pronunciations), then the statement regarding "sameness" risks the criticism of falsity. Although danced spaces are very different in some ways from signing spaces (as in American sign language, Plains Indian sign languages, etc.), one shared feature of the two kinds of systems exists in the fact that there is a conventionally acceptable latitude of performance of any given action sign or stretch of action signs which is, of course, the real justification of postulating constituent and contingent features of the system in the first instance.

  2. The entire focus of Allen's analysis is on gestural data alone. Readers are told nothing about the characteristics of the spaces in which the dancers moved. It is as if any given dance exists solely in the performed actions themselves, bearing no relation to the conceptual features of the spaces in which the patterns of the dance and the dancer(s) exist. For example, there is no information given about referential, directional, or locative features of the dances. We may well ask, "Are these dances based on a notion of cardinal directions, or some schema that represents an 'embedded' space within the geographical directions, or are the dances talked about spatially organized by the paradigmatic features of dreaming tracks?" Wild's analysis of Walpiri danced spaces (1977) is much more satisfactory from this standpoint.

  3. The writing of movement, regardless of the type of script used (that is, whether it is Benesh, Labanotation, or Eshkol-Wachmann, the three most viable scripts of movement writing extant today) can be, and with regard to human dances, must be, an entirely different exercise from that of writing human actions because

an intentional action is not the same as a physical movement since the latter can be described in various ways according to one's point of view and one's beliefs about the person performing it. One cannot specify an action, as opposed to a purely physical movement, without taking into account what the agent intended. (Best 1974: 193)

For a more detailed analysis of this problem connected with the writing of Luo danced actions (Africa), see Durr (1981), where the analysis also includes written samples of both modes of writing and the different results obtainable therefrom.

  1. It has to be said that Allen's article does not refer to "Australian Aboriginal Dance" as a whole. Hardly more than mention is made of six dances of four specific peoples in northern Australia: Wogaidj (Delissaville) and the "Buffalo," "Crab," and "Birdsong" dances; Maiali (Bamyili) and the "Fish" and "Black Crow" dances; Nunggubuyu (Rose River) and the "Brolga" dance; and Wanindilyaugwa (Groote Eylandt), where the author's comments are to the effect that there appears to be the same overall pattern and sequence for all dances. That may indeed have been the case, but if it is, then readers need to know why this is the case with Groote Eylandt dances and not with the others.

Allen's article would have been greatly improved by changing the title to "Six Dances of Four North Australian Peoples" because that is what the main dance content of the piece is about. There is no way that a fifteen-page article, which includes seven pages of photographic plates, can deliver the promise that the title  "Australian Aboriginal Dance" implies. For something approaching adequacy in relation to this kind of title, see Wild et al. (1998).Wild's article, in conjunction with the contributions of von Sturmer, Grau, Clunies-Ross, and Dail-Jones on specific areas in Australia, delivers considerably more of the promise of such a title. In fact, von Stunner's blunt remark, "The Aboriginal case? There is none—or in fact not one; only many" (1987: 65) is surely appropriate here.

     I do not deny that popularizations of materials about dances, Aboriginal and otherwise, are necessary, nor do I deny that the casual or occasional reader might want generalized information; but I do deny that such materials have to be written in such a way that they mislead readers and create more stereotypes than they dispel. It is all too easy, for example, to assume that "Aboriginal dancing" is somehow the same. The issue is an old one in anthropology: the relativist's position against various kinds of 'universalist' arguments. Specific to Australia is a homogenization (a kind of universalization) which leads people to believe that the didjeridu, a musical instrument unique to Arnhem Land and borrowed elsewhere, is (or was) used by all Aboriginal peoples. Generally in the past, the relativist rather than the universalist arguments had more force, simply because ethnographies have tended to be based on empirically perceivable elaborations of gestural and spatial patterns of a generic form of dancing found in a specified area.

     During the past seventeen years, research in other parts of the world has produced evidence that there is just cause to doubt that a broad classification of dancing—say, "jazz dancing" or "folk dancing" or "Aboriginal dancing"—possesses universal characteristics of need, function, form, meaning, motivation, or what you will. Forms of body language(s) seem to have developed independently; moreover, they can be shown irrevocably to be tied to the human faculty for language use. Simply put, Ga, Ewe, Asahanti, and Dagomba dancing, and the dances themselves, are very different from one another, although the peoples who generated the dance forms are all "Ghanaians" or "West Africans."7 Jazz dancing, ballet dancing, tap dancing, and square dancing are all performed by Americans, but they are not the same kind of dancing. There is no more reason for lumping Aboriginal dancing into one indistinguishable homogeneous mass either, yet that seems to be done in the literature, where one would never find comparable usages of the term American dancing or British dancing. Why?

Universals and 'Natural' Dancing

There do exist certain universal features of dancing, whether it is Aboriginal dancing or some other kind of dancing. Furthermore, there is a clearly discernible interface between the spoken (or 'natural') language of any given group of people and their body language(s). These universals are to be found in the locally Euclidean features of the spaces in which any danced action occurs—and for that matter, any human action at all; they are the dimensions of up/down, right/left, front/back, and inside/outside. There are structural universals pertaining to the human body as well, which will be mentioned later. These spatial and body-related universals are comparable to the universals of pitch, harmonics, and time which cause all human languages to cohere at a meta-theoretical level, but no one imagines that these universals are to be found in their pristine state in specific languages, specific pieces of music, or culture-specific dances. The point is that the notion of universal structures of dancing and human movement is at least as recondite and complex as is the notion of harmonics and pitch in music or phonetics or phonology in human spoken languages (see Williams 1976b and 1976c, for further elucidation). The problem for the serious student of danced forms of human movement is that the structures of human actions are not given the same status as are those which pertain to music and language, but this matter cannot detain us further here.

     With regard to the 'interface' alluded to above, one would point to recent work in linguistics, specifically to what Fillmore (1983: 317) called the "semantic primitives" (not in Wierzbicka's 1980 sense), that is, certain fixed, closed class and schematized devices within the syntax and morphology of the spoken language of any given people which characterize space, location, position, and direction (see Haviland 1986, for an excellent discussion of these with regard to Guugu Yimidhirr, a local Paman language spoken at the Hopevale Mission in southeastern Cape York Peninsula).

     Clearly, a consideration of universals in dancing (or any structured human system of actions whatsoever) leads to questions of great import. Just one of these is the problem of typologies, for it is with an attempt at a typology of dancing that Allen's article on Aboriginal dancing begins. The author lists what she believes to be all of the ways that dancing is used in an Aboriginal context, although just how she arrived at the list and why we should accept it as authoritative is not given. The four peoples to whom the six dances belong that are talked about in the article are characterized as persons to whom art forms, including the dance, comes "naturally."

     It is difficult to imagine what the author means by "natural" dancing, and it is unclear whether the dancing is "natural" because it is connected with many life activities—in which case, all danced forms are "natural"—or whether the source of the "naturalness" lies elsewhere, say, in some form of bodily logos, perhaps, that is universal to dancing but not to any other human action; or perhaps what she implies are any number of "origins" arguments and theories about dancing which have been postulated over the years. Statements about the "naturalness" of dancing generally point to unimaginative handling of the dances and to the fact that the investigator did not try to find out how the dances were learned, viewing a dance as an isolated artifact of the culture concerned, as if it existed in a conceptual, linguistic, spatial vacuum.

     Anthropologists and linguists have produced evidence that there are complex learning processes that accompany the learning of Aboriginal dancing, for example, Goodale (1971), Kaberry (1939), and Elkin (1972 and 1974 [1938]). Certainly it is the case that, often in West Africa (where dancing is also stigmatized as "natural," "unfettered," and "primitive"), many years are required for the formation of a dancer, as in the case of the seven years of singing, dancing, and drumming needed to produce a Kple priestess. It is as if investigators cannot recognize the process of learning if the paraphernalia of learning with which they are familiar is absent. For many years, African peoples were thought to have no law, simply because they had no courts; no government, because they had no rulers of the kind that Europeans were used to. Is it the case that Allen thought that Aboriginal dancing was "natural" because she saw no ballet barres, studios, mirrors, and pianos?

     In a thoughtful, perceptive article regarding the problems of ethnomusicological fieldwork in general, Ellis may supply part of the answer: "There is a lack of understanding of the work, a naive belief on the part of some authorities that all that is necessary for successful ethnomusicological work is the mere manipulation of a tape recorder" (Ellis 1970: 76).

     There is a similar lack of understanding of the nature of research into dances, a naive belief on the part of authorities and non-authorities alike that all that is necessary for research into dances is (1) a notator or choreologist who has danced—often in an idiom totally unrelated to that which is being investigated—and (2) a video camera or a film crew.

     One would want to add to this the problem of classification of body parts (see Ardener 1982 and Williams 1980a and 1980b).The human body is divided by different criteria in different languages; thus, a human action sign in contrast to a raw bodily movement has to be dealt with simultaneously in terms of classification, conceptualization, and action. Simply put, one would have to know what taxonomies of the body existed among the people Allen talks about and how the body parts are divided. Then the question of how these affect the performed actions in the dances arises, plus the elements of systems of reference and systems of address and their effects on the dances. Through these kinds of co-relations (their presence and/or absence), part of the significance of the danced figures in relation to the rest of the body language of the community begins to emerge. To explain and develop fully the many arguments and issues that are relevant to fieldwork problems and the dance would require another article at least as long as Ellis's, which is 136 pages long. Add to this the problems of the use of film in the field and the comprehension of film and videotaping as yet another 'languaging process' in that it involves significant selections, and it is easy to see how much writing about dances, including Allen's, is perhaps best recognized as a kind of simplistic gloss connected with realities which on the whole remain unexamined.

The Notion of Translation

Further to these points, readers would do well to reflect on Chapman's summarization of this kind of problem when he says,

There is not . . . any serious popular conception that such things require "translation" from one culture to another. Most people, when faced with an unintelligible foreign language, will recognize the need for "translation;" non-verbal "language" gestures, and generally semantic use of the body, of the person, or of groups of people, are not usually granted the same status as language in this respect. Translation will not be thought necessary. In general, an "English-speaker" will interpret the gestures of, say, a "Breton-speaker," a "French-speaker" or a "Gaelic-speaker," according to an entirely "English" set of rules of interpretation, without feeling any need to go to the bother of "translating." (Chapman 1982: 111)

Field investigators who are going to study the body language of any human group or any sub-set thereof are well advised, first, to refrain from taking into the field (in their own culture or that of another) the kinds of prejudices, naïvetés, biases, and simple ignorance of the nature of dance research which have consistently been demonstrated by many of their predecessors. If they choose to try to do fieldwork in the 1980s and 1990s carrying this kind of cultural baggage with them, then they will have to expect that savage criticisms will inevitably follow. Again, one turns to Stanner:

Modern anthropologists criticize their nineteenth-century predecessors for many faults, and the force of the criticisms is reflected in many aboriginal positions. . . . But a cardinal fault—the invincible ignorance about Aboriginal religion—has not been criticized sufficiently (1979: 111).

I am convinced that there is an "invincible ignorance" about human dancing, dances, and 'the dance' which has not been sufficiently criticized either. It is simply boring, apart from anything else, to be faced with the same old clichés again with regard to this little known and less understood activity. Since I intend to share some of the more commonly used theories of dancing with readers later on in this essay, I will say no more about the matter here.

     Instead, I would want to draw attention to some sentences, chosen at random from von Sturmer (1998) to illustrate the point I am trying to make. He says, "[A]ll dances, even those now secular, took root in the religious life." There is no awareness of this, hence no mention of it in Allen's article and in many others of the same genre. It cannot be over-stressed that the very language used to describe a dance either reveals or obscures the investigator's knowledge of the subjects of investigation. For example, talking about Cape York, von Sturmer says,

[I]ndividuals represent themselves in more or less heightened ways. They are not bonefish, taipan, blue-tongued lizard; they are Bonefish Man, Taipan Man, Blue-tongued Lizard Man. While they may exhibit some of the features of their eponymous animal species, they dance as men—or, perhaps more accurately, as spirits, or as revealing their own spiritual essence. These dances (which are fairly typical) bear little mimetic or even narrative load: they are representational in a highly formalised way, but of human activities and responses, personal identity, social relations, not animal behaviours. (1998: 1)

Allen, together with many other writers on Aboriginal dancing, when faced with "Brolga," "Buffalo," "Fish" or other "totemic" dances merely says that the dancers are mimicking the creatures. Someone is wrong. Either what von Sturmer says about the nature of these dances hits the mark, or it is wide of the mark and an imitation theory of human dancing is the more accurate. Thus, we are led to consideration of the nature of the human act of dancing—and a great deal hangs on these considerations, not only for Aborigines but for ourselves. Yet, many dance researchers will say, Australia and abroad, "All I want to do is to describe the dances. I am not interested in all the theoretical stuff."8 They seem to imagine that one can produce an ethnographic description that is void of theory—an impossibility, as we have seen. The fact is that the pitfalls inherent in alleged 'simple description' are legion!

     In any case, it simply does not do to talk of any form of Aboriginal dancing, whether it seems to the investigator to be a 'fun' dance or a 'ceremony,' solely in terms of metaphors drawn from the Western stage. Allen refers to bushes and trees in the physical spaces of the dances she saw as "set pieces," as if the dances were taking place on a stage in Sydney or Melbourne, and of objects used in the dance by the dancers as "props," a blatant case of an overlay of the conceptual spaces of Allen's familiar forms of dancing onto the conceptual spaces of dancers and dances to whom these notions are completely alien.

     In spite of the fact that movement notators, especially, are aware of the rules of the game in their own idioms of dancing and in the exercise of writing movement itself, they seem able to develop acute myopia when they are faced with idioms of dancing with which they are not familiar. It is as if they see no need for translation, no need for working out the rules of the body-language games of the dances under investigation. Clearly, there is no point in working out the rules of the game if the dances under investigation are thought to be 'spontaneous' and 'natural' (whatever that may mean). It is the case that dancing is learned, and there are far better theoretical and methodological frameworks for the study of it than a kind of wooly unilinear evolutionism or the pure, untouched romanticism of early functionalism, that is, "they-have-such-a-marvelous-sense-of-rhythm" syndrome.

Universals Again

At this point, I shall briefly sketch some of the universal features of dances and, indeed, of all human actions, anywhere in the world, so that readers might more readily comprehend the theoretical underpinnings of a different anthropological approach to the study of dances.

     It seemed reasonable to assume that it would be useful (1) to know what is in fact universal regarding the phenomenon of human movement and dancing in order that (2) we might possess an objective, scientifically valid foundation for cross-cultural generalization and comparison and (3) a useful set of open structures which are not initially data-laden, on a basis of which we might better understand how and in what ways the enormous variety of semantic elaborations of these structures has developed and been utilized by different peoples of the world.

     In other words, in order to talk about "Z" (specific dances of any culture(s) in any historical period anywhere in the world), one has to know something about "X" and "Y"—that is, the structural characteristics of the expressive human body (X) and the space(s) in which it moves (Y). These structures are not the dances themselves but consist of axiomatic statements about the human body and the space/time in which it moves, for example, "the elbow only has one degree of freedom" or "the human semasiological9 body [sema-siological = expressive or semantically laden] is a ninety-dimensional, self-activating, mobile object which operates in a locally Euclidean four-dimensional space/time" (see Williams 1975 for full discussion).

     These structures constitute, as it were, the rules of the rules of dancing and human actions. They are necessary for investigators to know but do not necessarily enter explicitly into the ethnographic description of any given danced form encountered in the field. They are encountered, however, at the 'interface' between the spoken language of a people and in the notion of the semantic primitives of up/down, right/left, front/back, and inside/outside. For example, about Guugu Yimidhirr, Haviland says that this language "seems to concern itself deeply with location, not only in the frequent practise of describing where things are, or in the universal need to locate referents" (1986: 1), but with regard to features of motion, position, spatial relations, and direction as well. He also points to other significant semasiological features of this spoken language when he says

There are some specific locational words as well. Hopevale people use a familiar contrast in two deictic roots (yi—'here,' 'this,' and nha—'there,' 'that') along with the roots bada 'down,' 'below,' and wangaar 'up,' 'above,' to locate and identify things. There are two further complex deictic words, both typically requiring a gesture to specify their meanings [underline supplied]: yarrba 'thus,' 'in this way,' a kind of demonstrative word, and yarra 'there,' 'that,' whose use implies: 'Have a look at that,' or 'There it is.' (Haviland 1986: 2)

Later, we are told that Guugu Yimidhirr's "repertoire of distinct locational or deictic roots is actually somewhat meagre and underspecified: a simple proximate/distal deictic contrast, supplemented by a basic, if hardworking, vertical opposition, and two further demonstratives which, as I say, typically require gestural supplementation" (Haviland 1986: 2).

     To a semasiologist, the locational, directional, demonstrative, and deictic features of a spoken language are some of the indices which, combined with ostensive evidentials (that scheme of deictics which places distances relative to an actor) plus all the empirically observable actions which accompany them, are the 'stuff,' so to speak, of which dances, rituals, rites, signing systems, or what you will are made. The point of this sketchy overview of some of the relations between empirically perceivable (transitive) structures and intransitive structures (non-empirically perceivable 'givens') is simply this: dances do not exist within a conceptual, a spatio-linguistic, or a cultural vacuum. It is, in our view, impossible to carry out effective, confirmable, cross-cultural comparison and to avoid a total relativist's position with reference to the study of Aboriginal or any other kind of dancing, without a sufficiently sophisticated theoretical structure with which to begin (see Williams 1980c, for further discussion).

     Moreover, without such knowledge, plus certain other features of each individual culture (as I have tied to indicate above)—which includes taxonomies of the body, deictic contrasts, and coordinates which are culture-specific, including systems of address, pronominal systems of reference, and such—we are ill placed to comprehend specifically human movement as it manifests itself in semantically laden actions, in contrast, say, to the movements of animals and birds or the sign functions of machines (see Williams 1986). Because of this, one proceeds from the axiom that human spatial points of reference are points of application for linguistic predicates, a summation of perceptions gained through reading Hampshire (1959). Boiled down further, the statement is meant to imply one of the significant differences between the human use of space as against the use of space by other sensate creatures: in the human instance, there is an irrevocable connection between spatial points of reference and the use of language. Simple examples are those explicated by Haviland (1986), the cardinal directions and any permutations of them, spatial metaphors—which abound in any human language—and the canonical coordinate system in which all human actions take place. As in physics, certain relationships between human bodies and the spaces in which they move must be known in advance.

     Such universals as I have pointed to are the 'gear,' so to speak, that (1) all moving human beings possess, and (2) any investigator needs to know in order to elaborate in any way—or to describe the elaborations which exist—with regard to any dance, sign system, or other structural system of meanings of an alleged 'non-verbal' nature. The reason why these are structural, not semantic, universals is that, for example, there are no grounds for saying that dancing is done for the same reasons everywhere in the world—or anywhere. One is not saying that movements have been found that people use for various purposes everywhere in the world (pace Fast 1970, Morris et al. 1979, and others).

     One can legitimately postulate certain structural and semantic primitives10 which are universal to every manifestation of body language anywhere in the world; indeed, Haviland implies (1986: 26n16) that McNeill (1979) and McNeill and Levy (1982), "seem to suggest that the conceptual structure that underlies gestural production is in some ways also the deepest structure that underlies spoken language as well." If that is the case, then the conceptual structures of human actions postulated by semasiology are those to which McNeill and Levy could be seen to refer. It is gratifying to know that colleagues in a sister discipline may have arrived, through independent types of investigation, at one or two of the same conclusions reached in Williams's doctoral work (1975) pertaining to dances and human actions, although I would not want this statement to be misconstrued as a dogmatic assertion that the conclusions are the same.

     There are certain similarities of approach to the notion of universals referred to as "etic" features of body languages (with specific cultural manifestations being the "emic" characteristics), and this is roughly the theoretical approach taken by Kaeppler (1972), following the intellectual developments of American ethno-scientists and the linguistic researches of Kenneth Pike. In my own frame of reference, we talk of "intransitive" and "transitive" aspects of human action sign systems. Dances, in this context, are seen as extremely powerful, generally very dense encapsulations of the semantic primitives mentioned above, simply because they are conventionalized forms of body language(s) which depend for their existence on implicit knowledges of these universals. It is thus not the case, as Grau would lead us to believe, that

[t]he distinction between dance and non-dance must be equivocal, because it depends upon culturally shaped as well as universally objective components. At this stage, we do not know what these universally objective components are, but it is possible to find out the culturally relative answers by isolating the appropriate ethnodomains. (1983: 32, italics added)

While one would agree with Grau that dances "depend upon culturally shaped as well as universally objective components," her assertion that we do not know what these universal components are is based on a narrow and incomplete reading of currently used theories of human actions and the dance.11

     Grau's positive contribution to the study of Tiwi dancing lies in the excellence of her data, in the meticulous manner in which it was handled, and in the insights she achieved which led her toward the study of the taxonomy of Tiwi bodies and their relation to the danced actions they perform. It is unfortunate that this author's doctoral dissertation lacks in theoretical underpinnings and orientation because the data are extraordinary—but it seems to lack organization into anything. We have yet to hear from Grau about the real design of Tiwi dancing and signing systems, and it is to be hoped that we are not kept too long in suspense, for she is in an admirably placed position, having freed herself from the strictures of a too-limiting supervision, to extend and further discipline her work into major post-doctoral contributions.

     In contrast, Dail-Jones's M.A. thesis has a theoretical framework; she advances an argument and follows it through, but her choice of theory leaves much to be desired, as it stems from hard-core functionalism and behaviorism (perhaps reflecting what her supervisors deemed to be 'scientific'), leading to statements like this: "For Walpiri women, dance as a whole consists predominantly of adaptors and illustrators interspersed with regulators; it often results in affect displays" (1984: 369).

     One can be sure that "for Walpiri women" dancing does not consist of anything of the kind.12 There is a disastrous confusion here among the investigator's home-made models of events (which generally get totally amended or completely discarded over the period of fieldwork), her analytical models, and the folk-models of the events being described. Inevitably, this kind of confusion results in nonsensical statements of the kind quoted above. Fortunately, Dail-Jones has an opportunity to redeem herself through work at a doctoral level where she could acquaint herself with more modern, certainly more rigorous, self-reflexive theorizing and with more anthropologically orientated approaches to the study of movement rather than behavioristic ones. She was hampered by supervision which came from outside the discipline, and this would not be important except for the fact that the scientistic theorizing she does use tends to obscure and obfuscate her data. This is unfortunate, because Dail-Jones tried to see relationships of other types of Walpiri-patterned movement and the danced movements, and no one but Dail-Jones has, up to now, tried to grasp the organization, specifically of women's dances.

     Having said that, it is necessary to digress a moment in order to recognize the admirable work of Diane Bell, whose ethnography Daughters of the Dreaming points to important features of the dances and ceremonies of women, although not in a specific sense. That work is reserved for another dimension of this anthropologist's applied approach to the study of central desert women. Unfortunately, much of Bell's work that is specific to the dance is restricted material and is contained in evidence which she helped women to present—in the form of dances and ceremonies—pertaining to land claims and rights. I can therefore cite only two pieces of unrestricted material in this essay and suggest that, for further information, the author herself be contacted for anything else which may be available (see Bell 1982 and 1983–84).

     With regard to the two theses mentioned above, it can be said that Grau's and Dail-Jones's works represent the first attempts in Australia scientifically to study human movement and the dance—with the exception of Kendon's work on sign languages (see Kendon 1983, for a good discussion of the kind of work it is, and see also Haviland 1986, for his comments on Kendon's theoretical stance). Grau's and Dail-Jones's theses represent a giant step forward from that which one can only call "pre-scientific" writers, for example, Allen (1973), Quisenberry (1973), Morse (1968), Dean (1955), Dean and Carell (1955), and Jones (1980). Dean is quoted by Morse as saying, "[R]esearch dancers using an accredited dance notation . . . are also needed" (Morse 1968: 3); and while it is true that people who have danced who use an accredited notation system are needed, it is equally true that dance experience alone is not enough, nor is the fact that dancers can usually learn the movements of other danced forms enough.

     The reasons for this are twofold: (1) idioms of dancing are analogous to spoken languages in important ways. The matter of learning a language other than one's own is a complex affair with regard to spoken language(s) and body language(s) alike. A fluent grasp of the phonological characteristics of one's native spoken language does not provide a sufficient basis for the understanding of a different language. Similarly, a fluent grasp of the kinological characteristics of, say, ballet dancing, does not provide a sufficient basis for the understanding and performance of a different danced idiom of body language.

     (2) Whether we like it or not, those of us who deal with so-called non-verbal materials are faced at the outset with major problems of translation, transliteration, and transcription: those of a space/time system, whether it is a dance, a rite, a ceremony, a system of greetings, or what you will, into spoken and, more accurately, into written language. These space/time systems occupy geographical spaces which are at once physical, social, semantic, and conceptual. We must use written language to communicate to others about the system, as we use spoken language to express the system, but we also know that spoken or written language introduces other things into the system. As Ardener has pointed out, conventional language intrudes itself into the system (1975), and it is simply a nonsense to imagine that it does not—or worse, attempt to ignore the fact that it does, perhaps underlining Wittgenstein's observation that it is easier to bury a problem than to solve it.

     The description of the danced actions of another people, whether in our own or another culture, is such a delicate affair, requiring such developed sensibilities of numerous kinds, that it is difficult to know where to begin to explain just what is needed for adequate preparation for the task. Suffice it to say here that one can, at least, now point to the works of better educated and trained professionals for guidelines (see endnote 1) for list of references.

Process and/or Preservation

Morse' s article documents an interesting and important period in the thinking about dancing (dances and "the" dance) in Australia: She was one of three people who carried out a project in the Northern Territory in the second half of the 1960s which was meant to test the efficacy of notating Aboriginal dances in the field. The project was endorsed by several distinguished scholars in the ethnomusicological and anthropological fields, among them Alice Moyle (1977a, 1977b, and 1978) and McCarthy (1957, 1964, and 1978), who was principal of the A.I.A.S. at the time. Morse says, "Our aim can be described simply: to obtain a precise record of Aboriginal dances in the form of a written score of both the dance movements and the music to which the dances are performed" (1968: 3–4).

     This kind of aim is heavily 'preservation' oriented, and there are many parallels in the Australian case with attempts which have been made over the past thirty years in China to preserve minority dances in that country. Space prevents teasing out the issues involved, but we possess an excellent account of the issues and the consequences of the approach in the Chinese context by Fairbank (1985 and 1986). With all due respect to the project in which Morse participated, it must be said that "totemic" dances show themselves to the modern investigator not as simple-minded mimed images of the physical environments of the peoples who generate them. If what von Sturmer says about Cape York dances is true (see above), then we can assume that the following statement is simply dead wrong:

[T]he dances we saw were mainly concerned with mimed description of everyday events, such as buffalo hunt, the catching of crabs and of fish. Some dances simply mimicked the activities of creatures such as birds, without human characters at all. (Morse 1968: 6)

Stanner put the matter in more generalized terms about as succinctly as possible:

[A]a 'totemic' system shows itself as a link between cosmogony, cosmology, and ontology; between Aboriginal intuitions of the beginnings of things and resulting relevances for men's individual and social being, and a continuously meaningful life. . . . Aboriginal totemic groups were thus sacred corporations in perpetuity. The yearly round of rites let the Aborigines renew both the sources and the bonds of life constituted in that way. (1979: 143)

Surely, "totemic dances," wherever they may take place in Australia, partake of all this, and dances that use an imagery that is based on vital and significant features of the environment (whether sacred or not) should command enough respect, one would have thought, for us to attempt to look beyond the signifier to the signified. Preservation of our own, or Aboriginal, culture may be a desirable aim, but it is neither the most important (taken in its 'museum' sense) with regard to dances nor is it the most fruitful, seen from the standpoint of a semantic anthropology (see Williams 1982).

     Dances provide us with encapsulations of ontological facts. Recognition of this is difficult if too much stress is laid on historicist or functionalist approaches. This is why it is so important to see human action sign systems, including dances, as kinds of languages: they can be notated: they possess syntax, grammars, and all the rest. They are reflexive, referential, and relational. They structure space. Their vocabularies and the degrees of freedom of their executants' bodies may be more or less articulate. Perhaps it is necessary to make the effort to see human action sign systems as 'language processes' which occur not in the medium of sound but in the medium of movement.

     The danger in an unenlightened 'preservationist' approach is, of course, that it focuses almost entirely on the 'vehicles'—on the 'products,' as it were—thus tending to disregard the content. It is possible to focus only on the movements and the ritual form in the event so that the meanings and substance of the ritual or dance are entirely forgotten. There is an unconscious separation of the signifier/signified unity, with a resulting distortion of the whole. This is why it is so important to stress the distinction between notating 'movement' as against 'actions.' It is why it is so important to know why one is undertaking any given investigation in the first instance.

Early Studies of Dancing

The first publication about dance that had any real relevance to anthropology was Curt Sachs's Eine Weltgeschichte des Tanzes, published in 1933 and translated into English in 1937 as World History of the Dance. This book has been widely used, and indeed is still used today, as a definitive anthropological study of dance. Although this book certainly has a place today in the study of anthropological theory, it has no place in the study of dance in anthropological perspective. Its theoretical stance is derived from the German Kulturkreis school of Schmidt and Graebner in which worldwide diffusion resulted in a form of unilineal evolution. But just as modern non-Western peoples do not represent earlier stages of Western cultural evolution, there is no reason to believe that non-Western dance represents earlier stages of Western dance. Yet some anthropologists find it possible to accept the latter without accepting the former (see Youngerman 1974, for more detailed discussion, and Williams 1976d, for a review of Lange [1975] which is a modern version of Sachs's theories).

Much more important for the study of dance in anthropological perspective, although he did not really address himself to the subject, was Franz Boas, whose orientation offers scope for analyzing dance as culture rather than using dance data to fit theories and generalizations. Boas felt that man had a basic need for order and rhythm—a need which Boas used to help explain the universal existence of art. By refusing to accept sweeping generalizations that did not account for cultural variability, he laid a foundation for the possibility of examining dance and responses to it in terms of one's own culture rather than as a universal language. In spite of Boas and others, however, the idea that dance (or art) can be understood cross-culturally without understanding an individual dance tradition in terms of the cultural background of which it is a part is not yet dead, especially among artists and dancers (Kaeppler 1978: 33).

     Just as Boas did not really address himself to the subject, neither did E. B. Tylor or Sir James Frazer address themselves directly to the subject. Students today are sometimes offended when they discover that Tylor referred to dancing as "frivolous and meaningless," and they find it difficult to understand why he was pessimistic about the future of dancing in modern civilization, why he thought that what remnants there were in England of folk dancing were dying out, that sportive dancing was falling off, and that, although sacred music was flourishing, civilization had mostly cast off sacred dance. "At low levels in civilization," he said, "dancing and play-acting are one" (1878: 15).

     Tylor's real interest in symbolic movement did not lie in its manifestations in dancing, but rather in the language of gestures used in deaf-signing. Tylor's work on gesture language is a rich and original source of linguistically based movement theory in anthropology (see Henson 1974, for fuller discussion and see Farnell 1984a, for recent work on Plains Indian sign language and American sign language). It requires much more than superficial handling, then, to accommodate his theories of gesture and movement to the dance as we understand it today without risking distortion, either of Tylor's thought or of the dance. But Tylor's name carries with it a cautionary tale: His thinking was in many ways a true reflection of the general evolutionary bias of nineteenth-century anthropology. Most of the writings about dancing during the period 1850–1900, whether contained in works on other subjects or whether they are solely about dancing, begin with sections or whole chapters on early Greek, Roman, or Egyptian dancing, or in the case of Lilly Grove (Sir James Frazer's wife), the emphasis is on 'primitive' dancing.

     For Frazer, dancing fit into a scheme of stages of an assumed human intellectual development: At the lowest end of the evolutionary continuum, the dance was placed as an exemplar of magic. Frazer thought that "primitives" called on magic when their capacity to deal with situations realistically was exhausted. Magic thus provided a substitute reality: If a tribe could not really make war on a neighboring village, then it could at least do a dance about it. In the Frazerian scheme of things, dancing was classified as sympathetic magic, and magic, of course, was wrong-headed science. The point here is that, in the intellectual battles that Frazer was really interested in fighting, he opposed both magic and science to religion. Although social anthropologists no longer accept Frazer's theory of stages of evolution or his assessments of dancing today, many artists, dancers, dance critics, and others still use these ideas. It is difficult to understand why. Keali'inohomoku states the problem succinctly:

Despite all [modern] anthropological evidence to the contrary, however, Western dance scholars set themselves up as authorities on the characteristics of primitive dance. Sorell (1967) combines most of these so-called characteristics of the primitive stereotype. He tells us that primitive dancers have no technique and no artistry, but that they are "unfailing masters of their bodies!" He states that their dances are disorganized and frenzied, but that they are able to translate all their feelings and emotions into movement. Primitive dances, he tells us, are serious but social. He claims that they have "complete freedom" but that men and women can't dance together [a 'fact' of sorts which Sorell may have gleaned from reading about central desert peoples in Australia]. He qualifies this statement by saying that men and women dance together after the dance degenerates into an orgy! Sorell also asserts that primitives cannot distinguish between the concrete and symbolic, that they dance for every occasion, and that they stamp around a lot! Further, Sorell asserts that dance in primitive societies is a special prerogative of males, especially chieftains, shamans and witch doctors. Kirstein also characterizes the dances of "natural unfettered societies" (whatever that means). (Keali'inohomoku 1980: 84)

Similar criticism could be leveled at most of the genre of 'dance books' that are commonly read and used throughout the English-speaking and European dance worlds: cf. Haskell (1960 and 1969), Kirstein (1924), DeMille (1963), Terry (1956 and 1967), Martin (1939 and 1963), and the many entries in dance encyclopedias under the headings "ethnic dance," "primitive dance," and "ethnologic dance." There are doubtless parallels in Australian literature on dancing; I have not yet read everything which has been written here, but I have seen the above-mentioned books on the shelves of bookstores, so one can fairly assume that they are there because people read them and swallow them, as it were, book, line, and thinker.

     Fortunately, anthropology has come a long way since the end of the nineteenth century:

Descriptions, brief as they are, of the dances are given from time to time to show that choreography is a real art with the Aborigines. Music, rhythm, actions and steps of both men and women dancers are worked out by the composer and master. It is not random or free activity. Directions are given by a leader, if details are not known. Behind each action is meaning. The pattern, however, does allow the virtuoso some latitude for improvising. (Elkin 1972: 275—originally written 1949)

Although the kinds of description which could be carried out today are far more rigorous than those used by Elkin, he nevertheless recognized the importance of the dances and music of the peoples he visited and the central place they hold as significant repositories of knowledge and mediators of meanings. The problem is that the writings of anthropologists like Elkin are rarely read by dance scholars or the general public, and it is to another such writer that we will now turn.

     Goodale's section of the book Tiwi Wives headed "Songs and Dances—The Yoi" (1971: 290–317) is one of the best I have read by an anthropologist who is not a dance and movement specialist. Part of the reason it is so good is that Goodale does not attempt to describe the movements of the dance as they occur—an enterprise that is fraught with difficulties, producing results which are generally unsatisfactory either because the investigator is forced to use unsuitable metaphors or because the process simply becomes unbearably tedious. So many parts of the body can move at the same time, that a verbal description (which by its very nature erases the simultaneity that is characteristic of danced movement) is rarely accurate in any case, hence the need for a script which can handle human actions.

     Instead, Goodale talks about the forms of dancing which occur in the pukamani ceremony (the burial, the ilanea or final grave rituals) and the kulama ceremony. Her work yields many insights with regard to Tiwi dancing: "Songs and dances are not only forms of creative art among the Tiwi, but they are perhaps the most important for gaining prestige" (1971: 290); "The culture hero, Purakapali, told the Tiwi 'to sing the things around them,' and so, like the kulama songs, the pukamani songs are about every subject imaginable to the Tiwi and are not traditional but individual compositions" (1971: 292–93); "but the singers and dancers were in fact 'marking' a particular animal because the associated dance was their yoi, their inherited dance form" (1971: 296); and finally,

The dances or yoi are as full of social implication as are the songs and are just as original and entertaining. I have in the preceding discussion of songs used the verb "to mark" in order to describe the symbolism of action or words referring to a particular subject. The Tiwi used this English word exclusively to describe what it was they were symbolizing in a song or dance, and I prefer to continue using this word (rather than "imitate" or "mimic") in order to emphasize the symbolic nature of these actions. What has been "marked" has been "emphasized," not merely imitated. (1971: 303)

Just one of the more puzzling features of Australian anthropology upon first encounter as an outsider is the varying usages of the terms 'totem' and 'totemic,' the meanings of which are, to say the least, elusive.13 These terms crop up, as we have seen, with great regularity with regard to dances (see Burridge 1973: 176–87, for a thoughtful discussion). One of the more interesting and potentially significant doctoral theses which could be undertaken in an anthropology of dance and human movement consists of an analysis of the concept of 'totem' in relation to the dances of one or more Aboriginal peoples.

The Notion of 'Theory' Again

There are other significant topics that also await investigation. Some of them turn around obvious problems in the literature on dancing with the notion of 'imitation' or 'mimicry' regarding dances. The imitation theory of dancing has had a long life in the intellectual history of Western civilization with reference to visual, graphic art, and dancing. Attempts at real analysis and the teasing out of an ideology of creativity seems to be lacking,14 yet we do not lack starting points, one of which might be found in the controversial writings of Ananda Coomeraswamy (1934, 1948, and 1956). Although he writes about the dances and arts of India, he nevertheless presents arguments that were meant to stimulate thought and research on the transformational processes involved in the creation of symbolic forms.

     Is it perhaps the case that there is more similarity between Aboriginal creative processes and those of other historical periods in our own—or Indian—history? Maybe the painters of religious icons in pre-Renaissance times more closely approximated the kinds of processes involved in the development of dances and rituals from the basis of the dreaming. It would be worth looking into, not because it would necessarily assist Aborigines in the short run in this case, but such an exercise might go some way toward assisting us better to understand (and therefore to appreciate and respect) what is uniquely a feature of their different orientations to cosmology and religion.

     Western art has not always—or even for very long—been dominated by its present tendencies toward a cult of the individual, 'pop' art, and various forms of deconstructionism. I would venture to guess that it would be extremely difficult to comprehend the significance of much traditional art in Australia or in the rest of the world in terms of these itinerant, relatively superficial, and generally shot-lived schools of thought.

[A]a fifteenth century painting is the deposit of a social relationship. On the one side there was a painter who made the picture, or at least supervised its making. On the other side there was somebody else who asked him to make it, provided funds for him to make it and, after he had made it, reckoned on using it in some way or other. Both parties worked within institutions and conventions—commercial, religious, perceptual, in the widest sense social—that were different from ours and influenced the forms of what they together made. (Baxandall 1972: 1)

     The elements of body language—gestures, postures, and such—that are depicted in the paintings Baxandall discusses are very different indeed from those to which we are accustomed. It requires equal efforts of understanding to comprehend these as it does to work out the elements of body language(s) of other cultural groups in whatever form they appear. Body language, in whatever form it may be depicted, is not a static, universally understood set of gestural or danced meanings which somehow remain constant while everything else in a culture moves and changes. Although it will come as a shock to some, there is no universally understood Esperanto of gesture and meaning.

Since the fifteenth century in Western cultures, a large number of notation systems for movement have appeared and disappeared (over eighty-seven notation systems for dance alone). Many of these have been little used because they were devised to meet the needs of one particular movement system, dance style, or research project and could not be generalized. It is only in the twentieth century that movement writing systems have emerged which can serve a great variety of applications. The problem has been one of developing a script that will preserve the identity of the movement, make possible accurate reproduction, and maintain semantic content. (Farnell 1989)

     These observations, written by Farnell for an International Encyclopedia of Communications (1989), indicate in a closely action-oriented fashion what some of the problems of notation, preservation, and recording of dancing and sign languages amounts to. And there is more: Farnell also says in her entry, titled "Body Movement Notations," that "[d]efinitions of human movement necessarily affect definitions of what being human amounts to—and these have changed considerably in Western cultures according to disciplinary and theoretical perspectives and as a result of historical changes in general intellectual climate."

     I cite the work above to draw attention to the fact that the biggest single obstacle, if it can be so characterized, to the notion of serious work in the field of an anthropology of dance and human movement lies in the long-held premises about mind/body relationships in Western cultures: the institutionalization of Christianity and, even more important, the Cartesian split between mind body and, therefore, of the body and the movements it makes. Some of the most deeply held prejudices in English-speaking cultures about these matters manifest themselves in relation to the subject of dancing and in the defining categories into which all dancing is classified. A lesser, but no less aggravating, problem for the serious researcher is the insistence by many on a final appeal to 'common-sense' thinking about dancing. Yet, it is common-sense thinking which labels Aboriginal dancing "stone-age dancing;" it is common sense to see all dancing as a stronghold of uncontrolled emotion, sinful desires, corrupting appetites, and private irrationalities of an astonishing variety. The fact that these commonsensical conclusions are based (maybe) on one or two kinds of dancing or that they stem from little exposure to the richness and variety of the danced forms of body languages throughout the world is ignored. It is from 'common sense' that the notion that dancing is 'primitive' grew—and still grows.

     If Stanner is to be believed, that "[i]t would be helpful to stop thinking of Aborigines as a 'primitive' people" (1979: 59), then we are also going to have to convince ourselves that it would be helpful to stop thinking of dancing as 'primitive' too: not just their dancing, but ours. It would not be an exaggeration to say that the only form of Western dancing which has consistently escaped the primitive stereotype is classical ballet. The relevant question is not why this is so but what is the history of various segments of Western civilization that has caused this idiom of dancing to escape the classification that all other forms of Western dancing and 'ethnic' dancing have been subjected to.15 A rather simplistic explanation but one which has some element of truth in it turns around the notions of an academy, a syllabus, and a more or less universalized technique and practice structure for the idiom. Because other forms of dancing lack these, they are thought to be more 'primitive,' hence less 'advanced' or 'developed' than the ballet. Insufficient attention has been paid to the historical development of the ballet and the reasons why it holds the privileged status that it does in English-speaking societies.

     There is a sense in which I am suggesting that our understanding of the dance is (and must be) relational; that is, what we think we know is always contingent upon that which we do not know. This raises several questions, some of which have been previously discussed, like the question of whether or not a choreologist who is solely trained in the idiom of ballet can adequately notate the dances of an unfamiliar idiom of dancing. There is no discussion of such questions in available literature at all. It has been my experience through the teaching of an anthropology of dance and human movement that we know very little about Western forms of dancing or why we view them the way we do; thus, the task of raising our collective consciousness about Aboriginal dancing or any other form of dancing other than our own is necessarily going to involve considerable effort toward understanding our own forms of dancing as well. Without this dual, self-reflexive process, we are going to be in a poor position to make any generalizations that are worthy of attention about the act of dancing in any culture, far less the entire world. And 'dance researchers' or 'dance scholars,' whether they are conscious of it or not, are going to continue to offer analyses or commentaries on the dances of other peoples solely in terms of the defining categories of the dominant culture, simply because, without education and training, they cannot do anything else.

Questions

Familiarity, gained through a lifetime's reading of most of the significant writings about the dance in English-speaking societies (which represents merely a segment of a notional world literature on the subject), has yielded evidence that there are a set of stock answers to the often-asked question, "Why do people dance?" Stated in minimal terms they are these:

  1. They dance because they want to have fun and relax—the dance is basically a vehicle for leisure and entertainment;

  2. They dance because of biological, organic, or instinctive needs of some kind—the dance as a precursor to spoken language or as an atavistic, primitive expression of the animal side of human nature;

  3. They dance because they want to express themselves—the dance as a symbolic activity divorced from real life;

  4. They dance because they feel sexy, happy, or sad or something—the dance as a prime repository of emotions (and in a world of logical positivism, these emotions have very little to do with rationality or with the human capacity for language use);

  5. They dance to show off or to relieve their over-burdened feelings—the dance as catharsis or as one of the governors on a stem-valve theory of human emotions;

  6. They dance because it is an innate, genetically programmed activity and because they refuse to give up that which represents a throwback to their ape-like ancestors;

  7. They dance because they cannot speak or write very well—the dance as a pre-literate phenomenon which carries the implication that learning to speak and write is going to put a stop to the dancing;

  8. They dance because they are dedicated to some notion of bodily praxis as against a dedication to some notion of verbal praxis—the dance as a more truthful, more integrated, more honest way of life;

  9. They dance because dancing is really a form of play—the dance as an activity belonging to the species homo ludens;

  10. They dance because they are performing some social function which contributes to the larger society (although just what this is is rarely ever explained except in terms like 'harmony' or some other term);

  11. They dance because they are really magicians or escapists of some kind––the dance as illusion, along with the rest of the theatrical professions;

  12. They dance because a spirit has possessed them, whether good or evil—the dance as an hysterical, neurotic, or quasi-religious manifestation;

  13. They dance because of an over-accumulation of sex hormones.

All of the above are documented in Williams (1991) to which this survey was (originally) attached, and the list I have given is by no means complete. However, a baker's dozen of them by way of illustration is enough to permit us to get on with the discussion.

     To that end, it is necessary to examine two difficulties with the old printed record. All of the answers above are inadequate, not only with reference to the nature of something as mind-boggling as the notion of the 'the dance' on a global scale but with reference the extent to which local definitions, reasons, motivations, and such are applied in the literature to the notion of 'the dance' on a worldwide scale. The fact is that "why do people dance?" as a viable question at this point in history is an exhausted question because it has prevented us from finding out about other aspects of the subject. If it is true that a legitimate response to the Kantian challenge of "what can we know?" lies in the prior question, "what can we ask?" then what can be known by any of us about the dance has an intimate relations with what is asked.

     There are so many unanswered questions: What are people doing when they dance? How is that dance put together so that is accomplishes the teleological ends for which it is designed? What is the conceptual space of that dance and how far does it compare with the conceptual spaces of "X" dance (sign system or whatever)? How far is gesture and human action influenced by taxonomies of the body, whether it is danced or non-danced human action? What is the relationship of the danced actions of "X" people with the pronominal systems of reference and the gestures of greeting in the same society? What are the implications and consequences of the proposition "movement is a literate medium of human expression"? Is there a difference between actions described as "signal," those described as "symbolic," and those described as "symptomatic"? What are the effects of the tourist industry on traditional dancing? What is meant by 'artifactualizing' a dance if the people who own the dance do not have a defining category of 'art'?

     As I have said elsewhere (Williams 1980a), the difficulty is that, where we pre-suppose a real level of "language" with regard to speaking, we do not tend to pre-suppose a similar abstraction when we speak of dancing or any other system of body language. The result is that the whole area of movement study is vitiated by generalizations that stem from comparatively limited notions about specific and minor uses of the medium of movement. Then, too, we often seem to be the unconscious victims of our own defining categories, definitions, and models of the role of movement in human societies. We divide actions (but not words) into 'symbolic' and 'instrumental' categories. Where we are familiar with polysemy, homonymy, and synonymy with reference to words, we seem to want to regard gestures as if they are in some sense semantically universal, even in the face of evidence that this is not at all the case. But it is to some different questions about dances and human movement that we will now turn, because there are examples in the Australian literature of attempts to enlarge the theoretical and methodological horizons of the field somewhat—or to undertake a different model of analysis entirely.

Different Questions

Borsboom (1978) was concerned with the apparent shift of a major rite of passage, the Maradjiri, in Arnhem Land. The ceremony he investigated looked as if it had brought about a break with Maradjiri rites, causing the new rite to be an exact opposite of the old one (Borsboom 1978: 172). Through extended fieldwork, this was proved not to be the case. Instead, it was found that (1) the internal structures of the rite remained and (2) elaborations were introduced of already existing features of the rite. There was also evidence of a rearrangement of existing symbolic elements of the rite, that is, the dreaming 'cluster' to which it belonged (1978: 172).

     The refreshing thing about Borsboom's work is that we see dances discussed in an overall context of a total Aboriginal cycle of life in the area, a singularly van Gennepian approach which in my view is a definite 'plus.' The major positive contribution of Borsboom's work, specifically with regard to dances, is the attention paid to overall patterns of the dances (1978: 90–129). These insights are offered: (1) "By means of traditionally stylized dances the actors re-enact the important Dreamtime exploits of their clan heroes, thus following up the instructions of what they usually call 'our Dreaming' (Sugar Bag). This Being showed how to sing and dance and in this way created the natural species and phenomena which now belong to the Wurgigandjar clan" (1978: 90); (2) "A series of dances of a particular Wurgigandjar ceremony, however, does not comprise all the dreamings which differs for every ceremony, and it is from such a selection of a certain number of dreamings—and the omission of others—that a clan ceremony such as the Maradjiri derives its definite character" (ibid.).

     There are eight dreamings danced in the Maradjiri ceremony, and instead of falling into the trap of trying to give a blow-by-blow description of the movements done by the dancers, Borsboom gives a detailed analysis of the spatial patterns thereof, the kin relations involved, and such. The author is straightforward about his own limitations—a feature of his work that is greatly to be admired. He says, for example, "No special attention will be paid to the dancing patterns of the women as I have found it impossible to observe properly their complicated and very stylized movements of feet, legs, arms, hands and head. They perform certain subtle variations for every dreaming, which will be mentioned in so far as possible" (1978: 95).

     Borsboom's conclusions are interesting: rite and myth, he tells us, "have the capacity to absorb history, but at the same time history intrudes upon rite and myth and brings about changes in composition and content" (1978: 183). He quotes Pouwer, whose article on signification and fieldwork (1973) is a standard reference for anthropologists of human movement. His work might have benefited by consulting Pocock (1967), as he is concerned with apperceptions of time. His work offers an intriguing and well documented basis for further work by a trained anthropologist of human movement who might well begin by asking, "What is the relation of the missing women's movements to the Maradjiri dance cycle as a whole?" It would be useful to possess notated scores of this dance cycle as well (both men's and women's parts), but that must await developments in the field of study to which I alluded at the beginning of this essay.

     Very different questions are being asked by two other researchers with regard to field studies already carried out in Arnhem Land: Stephen Wild and Margaret Clunies-Ross. Their current research seeks to answer the following questions:

  1. What are the rules governing the sequences of melodic and textual phrases in Djambidj songs?

  2. What are the rules governing the sequences of dance movement performed with the Djambidj songs?

  3. What effect does the presence or absence of dancing have on the rules for melodic and textual elements of performances?

  4. How are all of these elements (melody, text, dancing, stick patterns, didjeridu patterns, and dance calls) coordinated in performance?

  5. To what extent are the dreaming subjects of Djambidj differentiated by the elements of performance?

  6. To what extent do the contexts of occasions of performance determine the nature of performance?

     Wild's and Clunies-Ross's research is being conducted by notating all of the elements of a performance: classifying and coding the musical and textual phrases, danced movements, homogeneous stretches of stick beats and didjeridu sounds and discrete 'bands' of ritual calls, and entering these coded sequences into a computer for analysis of syntagmatic and paradigmatic patterns. Notations are being made from audio and video recordings of performances spanning about twenty-five years. The recorded data are complemented by extensive field observations and discussions with performers.

     The specific results that may be obtained from this research are not predictable at this stage, but whatever results may accrue, the project will be invaluable, especially from a kinological point of view, for no work of this kind has been undertaken before in Australia. Answers to the questions above will establish that Djambidj songs and dances have rules for a start. I think that the research will help to implant the idea that it demands a different approach to fieldwork than the traditional anthropological approach consisting of one ethnographer to one people. Any approach to the notion of a 'performance' which is based on consideration of music only, dancing only, language only is a reductionist's approach which fails to see the notion of performance as an integrated whole. Clunies-Ross and Wild seek to reintegrate their data at a different level in the hope that doing so will make possible different kinds of generalizations.

     There can be little doubt that different kinds of generalizations and fresh approaches to the Australian material are needed, both with regard to Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal dancing, sign languages, and non-vocalized systems of human actions of all kinds. It has been argued that not enough attention has been (or is being) paid to performances (see von Sturmer 1987: 74) which go beyond conventional social scientific styles of analyses—an appeal, one assumes, for a recognition of dances as le fait totale in the classic Maussian sense. One can agree only that the meaning of any given performance of dancing cannot be reduced to the mechanical playing out of sign systems, because one can exhaust the signs and yet know that there is something "over and above" (von Sturmer 1987: 74). But it is just this notion of "over and above" which has been very difficult to deal with in the past. Perhaps it is the case that excellent scholarship can only hope to point the finger (as in the Zen saying), restraining itself from the arrogance of confusing the pointing finger and the moon and accepting the fact of mysteries and beauties that cannot be captured in words in any case but only in silence.

     On a more mundane level, it can be said that behaviorism, for example, has demonstrably failed to produce insights into dances considered as 'behaviors'; phenomenology (as welcome as it was as a reaction against radical behaviorism and hardcore empiricism) still denies the dance any ontological existence in time (see Sheets 1966). The existence of 'spirit' can be no more successfully proved than the existence of 'psyche' in the tradition of post-Kantian metaphysics; thus, it has been to off-shoots of the recent linguistic revolution in the sciences to which some modern researchers have turned—with precisely what results it is as yet too early to say. At least the shift in philosophical thinking, best represented in the work of Best (1978 and 1985), and the shift in British anthropology from function to meaning (see Crick 1975) have opened new possibilities of theoretical and methodological approach, just as ethno-science did in the American anthropological tradition (see Kaeppler 1972, 1985, and 1986).

Conclusion

I should like to finish this essay by making three things about the scholarly study of dancing in Australia as plain as possible. Up to now, it is not the case that people have looked at dancing, Aboriginal or otherwise, without being able to see it. As I have tied to illustrate, through a balanced presentation of criticism and commendation, there are several writers who have 'seen,' and they have described, analyzed, and discussed their particular areas of interest very well. There are also those who have seen the need in a multi-cultural context for establishing scholarly studies of the dance in a hospitable academic atmosphere, making out of such study a legitimate area of serious research. Many friends of the subject have worked for many years, both within and outside universities, to change the perspective on the subject in several different ways.

     Internationally, the printed record, with regard to the anthropology of the dance and human movement, has improved remarkably in the past two decades. Australia has a unique contribution to make but cannot make it unless the subject is able to take its place along with music, theater, and the visual arts in higher-educational institutions. No one seems adequately to be able to explain the obvious disparities between the lack of available university qualifications in this country for the subject of the dance and the richness of opportunities of this kind available in connection with other art forms. It has been tentatively suggested that there is not sufficient literature to bear the weight of full-scale intellectual scrutiny. I hope that I have indicated by this writing that this is not the case.

     Second, it is as clear to me now as it was in 1970, when I commenced the study of social anthropology, that intimate knowledge of one, two, three, or more idioms of dancing does not automatically produce the ability to write about (or notate) the known idioms of dancing, far less those which may be unknown (see Williams 1976a). If any of us who have persisted with the subject through doctoral levels of study could have foreseen the extraordinary confusions which existed (and still do exist) about the subject of dancing in English-speaking societies, we might have felt despair and have never undertaken further study at all. Our personal odysseys, taken together, seem somewhat less radical now than they did, because the issues that were so vital then (circa 1965) have been replaced by others. The real problems to which I alluded at the beginning of this essay pertain not to whether the subject is viable but how the subject is to be incorporated into a university context in Australia. Nearly a decade has passed since the idea of a graduate degree in the anthropology of dance and human movement came into anyone's mind and began to be implemented through A.I.A.S. 'Real' problems now consist of educating and training young Australians of Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal descent to the task of coping with the literature, the theoretical and methodological problems that they inherit in an Australian context, and of encouraging them to contribute to the field at local, national, and international levels.

     Third, a new volume of essays on Songs of Aboriginal Australia (Clunies-Ross et al. 1987) has just been released which contains discussions about dancing, because the two phenomena of singing and dancing are very closely if not irrevocably linked in that context. In this collection of works by reputable scholars, I have already found clues to some possible answers to the question "What is a reasonable estimate of the present theoretical position(s) concerning the subject of dancing in Aboriginal Australia?" Whatever else may be said of this book by its future reviewers, it represents a significant contribution to the Australian literature on dancing because of its theoretical content and the clarity with which its authors and editors address some of these issues. It is a provocative and interesting set of essays which will provide future students with an invaluable intellectual foil as they attempt to sharpen their wits in preparation for their entry into an arena of discourse that is often contentious, certainly stimulating, and rarely without genuine interest and satisfying rewards.

Appendix A

(Excerpts from Review of Arts Education and Training, October 1985, Australian Government Publication Service, Canberra, 67–68.)

PROVISION IN DANCE

6.9       There is comparatively little tertiary provision in dance. Table 3 sets out the distribution of institutions with offerings in dance by State, level, and type of institution.

Table 3: DANCE--Distribution by State, Level of Course, and Type of Institution in 1984

State

Higher Degree

Post Graduate

Bachelor Degree

Diploma

Associate Diploma

 

CAE

UNI

CAE

UNI

CAE

UNI

CAE

UNI

CAE

UNI

NSW

-

-

-

-

1**

-

-

-

3

1**

VICTORIA

-

1*

-

-

1

1#

1

-

-

-

QUEENSLAND

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

1

-

WA

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

1

-

SA

-

-

-

-

1

-

-

-

1

-

TASMANIA

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

ACT

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

NT

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

-

TOTAL

-

1

-

-

3

1

1

-

6

1

*This is a research-based Masters degree in the Performing Arts. It is not a discrete dance course, nor is the degree course marked #.
**Minor strand only offered

6.9       There is only one course in dance at diploma level and four at degree level although in one only a minor strand is offered. For the most part courses are only available at the associate diploma level. Courses are offered by a total of six colleges of advanced education: two in New South Wales and one each in Victoria, Queensland, Western Australia and South Australia. Dance is available as a minor study within an associate diploma course offered by the University of Wollongong and as an element within courses in the performing arts offered by Deakin University.

6.10     A number of institutions are proposing to expand their offerings in dance during the next few years. The Victorian College of the Arts proposes to introduce a post-graduate diploma course in 1986 and degree courses are planned for introduction in Queensland, Western Australia and New South Wales.

Appendix B

(From: Wild, Stephen. 1986. Australian Aboriginal Theatrical Movement. Theatrical Movement: A Bibliographical Anthology [ed. B. Fleshman]. Metuchen, NJ: Scarecrow Press, 610–20.)

Allen, Elphine
     
1973. Australian Aboriginal Dance. In The Australian Aboriginal Heritage, an Introduction Through the Arts (ed. Ronald M. Berndt and E. S. Phillips). Sydney: Australian Society for Education through the Arts, in association with Ure Smith: 275–90.

Angas, George F.
     
1967a [1847]. A Savage Life and Scenes in Australia and New Zealand. In Landmarks in Anthropology (ed. Weston LaBarre). New York, London: Johnson Reprint Corp. Originally published by Smith, Elder, and Co., London.
     
1967b [1847]. South Australia: Illustrated (facsimile edition). Sydney: A.H. and A.W. Reed. Originally published by Thomas McLean, London.

Anonymous
     
1971. Mowanjum Dance Group. Department of Native Welfare Newsletter 1(10): 28–39.
     
1972. The Yelangi and Waiben Dancers. Identity 1(5): 18–23.
     
1975. Cairns Festival. Identity 2(6): 19–22.

Barlow, Alex
     
1980. Aboriginal Studies Resource List. Canberra: ALAS.

Basedow, Herbet
     
1925. The Australian Aboriginal. Adelaide: Preece.

Bates, Daisy M.
     
1905–6. The Marriage Laws and Some Customs of the West Australian Aborigines. Victorian Geographical Journal 13–14: 36–60.
     
n.d. Songs, Dances, etc.—Corroborees and Songs, Murchison. Unpublished ms. deposited in National Library of Australia, Canberra; copy deposited in AIAS, Canberra.

Bell, Diane.
     
1980. Daughters of the Dreaming. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Australian National University, Canberra.

Berndt, Catherine H.
     
1950. Women's Changing Ceremonies in Northern Australia. L'Homme 1. Paris: Hermann et Cie.
     
1962a. A Drama of North-Eastern Arnhem Land. Oceania 22: 216–39, 275–89.
     
1962b. The Arts of Life: An Australian Aboriginal Perspective. Westerly 1(2–3): 82–88.
     
1963. Art and Aesthetic Expression. In Australian Aboriginal Studies (ed. Helen Sheils). A symposium of papers presented at the 1961 research conference. Melbourne: Oxford University Press: 256–77.
     
1965. Women and the 'Secret Life.' In Aboriginal Man in Australia, Essays in Honour of Emeritus Professor A. P. Elkin (ed. Ronald and Catherine Berndt). Sydney, Melbourne, London: Angus and Robetson: 238–82.

Berndt, Ronald M.
     
1951. Kunapipi. Melbourne: Cheshire.
     
1952. Djanggawul. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
     
1974. Australian Aboriginal Religion. In Iconography of Religions, section V (ed. Th. P. van Baaren, L. Leetouwer and H. Buning). Leiden: E.J. Brill.

Berndt, Ronald M. and Catherine H. Berndt
     
1942–45. A Preliminary Report of Field Work in the Ooldea Region, Western Southern Australia. Oceania 12: 305–30; 13: 51–70, 143–69, 243–80, 362–75; 14: 220–49; 15: 239–75.
     
1951. Sexual Behaviour in Western Arnhem Land. New York: Viking Fund Publications in Anthropology, no. 16.
     
1962. Aborigines: Dancing. In Australian Encyclopaedia. 2d ed. Sydney: Grolier Society: 1: 62–64.
     
1970. Man, Land and Myth in North Australia: The Gunwinggu People. Sydney: Ure Smith.
     
1977 [1964]. The World of the First Australians. 2d ed. Sydney: Ure Smith.

Black, Lindsay
     
1944. The Bora Ground: Being a Continuation of a Series on the Customs of the Aborigines of the Darling River Valley and of Central New South Wales. Sydney: Booth.

Borsboom, Adrianus
     
1978. Maradjiri: A Modern Ritual Complex in Arnhem Land, north Australia. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Katholieke Universiteit, Nijmegan.

Campbell, Tomas D.
     
1940. The Drama and Theatre Arts of the Aborigines. Mankind 2(9): 329–30.

Chase, Athol
     
1980. Which Way Now? Tradition, Continuity and Change in a North Queensland Aboriginal Community [Lockhart River]. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, University of Queensland, Brisbane.

Church, A.E.
     
1945. Ballet at Badu. Walkabout 11(9): 33–34.

Clunies-Ross, Margaret and Stephen A. Wild
     
1981. The Relations of Music, Text and Dance in Arnhem Land Clan Songs. Paper presented at the Conference on Transmission in Oral and Written Traditions, Humanities Research Centre, Australian National University, Canberra, August 1981. [Published in 1984 as "Formal Performance: The Relations of Music, Text and Dance in Arnhem Land Clan Songs," Ethnomusicology 28(2): 209–35.]

Coppell, W.G.
1978. Audio-Visual Resource Material relating to the Aboriginal Australians. Canberra: Curriculum Development Centre.

Curr, Edward M.
     
1883. Recollections of Squatting in Victoria: Then Called the Port Phillip District (from 1841–1851). Melbourne: Robertson.
     
1886–87. The Australian Race. 4 vols. Melbourne: Ferres.

Daley, Charles
     
1925. Reminiscences from 1841 of William Kyle, a Pioneer. Victorian Historical Magazine 10(3): 158–72.

Dawson, James
     
1981 [1881]. Australian Aborigines: The Language and Customs of Several Tribes of Aborigines in the Western District of Victoria, Australia. Canberra: AIAS. Originally published by George Robertson, Melbourne, Sydney, Adelaide.

Dean, Beth
     
1955. In Search of Stone-Age Dance. Walkabout 21(5): 15–20.

Dean, Beth and Victor Carell
     
1955. Dust for the Dancers. Sydney: Ure Smith.

Durack, Mary
     
1971. No Longer Just a Dream: The Aboriginal Theatre Foundation Gets to Work. Identity 1(2): 17–22.

Edwards, Gregson
     
1971. Dancing for the Future. Northern Territory Affairs 2: 14–16.

Elkin, Adolphus Peter
     
1972. Two Rituals in South and Central Arnhem Land [Yabuduruwa and Maraian]. Sydney: University of Sydney. Oceania Monographs no. 19. Originally published in Oceania 31(3): 166–209; and 31(4): 259–93.
     
1974 [1938]. The Australian Aborigines. 5th ed. Sydney: Angus and Robertson.

Ellis, Catherine
     
1970. The Role of the Ethnomusicologist in the Study of Andagarinja Women's Ceremonies. Miscellanea Musicologica 5: 76–208.

Ewers, John K.
     
1954 [1953]. With the Sun on My Back. 2d rev. ed. Sydney: Angus and Robertson.
     
1964 [1947]. Aboriginal Ballet. In Walkabout's Australia: An Anthology of Articles and Photographs from Walkabout Magazine (ed. Alec T. Bolton). Sydney: Ure Smith in association with the Australian National Travel Association: 62–75. Repinted from Walkabout 14 (2): 29–34.

Eyre, Edward J.
     
1964 [1845]. Manners and Customs of the Aborigines of Australia. In his Journals of Expeditions of Discovery into Central Australia. Australiana Facsimile Editions no.7. Adelaide: Libraries Board of South Australia: II: 145–507. Originally published by T. & W. Boone, London.

Fink, Ruth A.
     
1960. The Changing Status and Cultural Identity of Western Australian Aborigines: A Field Study of Aborigines in the Murchison District, Western Australia, 1955–1957. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Columbia University, New York.

Goodale, Jane C.
     
1971. Tiwi Wives: A Study of Women of Melville Island, Northern Australia. Seattle: University of Washington Press.

Goodale, Jane and Joan D. Koss
     
1966. The Cultural Context of Creativity among Tiwi. American Ethnological Society Proceedings: 175–91.

Haddon, Alfred C.
     
1893. The Secular and Ceremonial Dances of Torres Straits. Internationales Archive für Ethnographic 6: 131–62.

Hassell, Ethel
     
1975. My Dusky Friends: Aboriginal Life, Customs and Legends and Glimpses of Life at Jarramungup in the 1880s; With an Introduction by Sara Meagher. Fremantle, Western Australia: C.W. Hassell.

Hiatt, Betty
     
1966. Report on the Female Aboriginal Dancing Associated with the Bora Ceremony at Lockhart River Mission in the Cape York Peninsula. Unpublished ms. deposited in AIAS, Canberra.

Hiatt, Lester R.
     
1965. Kingship and Conflict: A Study of an Aboriginal Community in Northeast Arnhem Land. Canberra: Australian National University Press.

Hodgkinson, Clement
     
1845. Australia from Port MacQuarie to Moreton Bay. London: T. & W. Boone.

Howitt, Alfred W.
     
1844. On Some Australian Ceremonies of Initiation. Royal Anthropological Institute Journal 13: 432–59.
     
1855. The Jeraeil or Initiation Ceremonies of the Kurnai Tibes. Royal Anthropological Institute Journal 14: 302–25.
     
1904. The Native Tribes of South-East Australia. London: Macmillan & Co.

Jones, Mary
     
1979. Dancing Feet? The History of Sacred Dance. Zadoc Central News, October 1979: 13–25.
     
1980. The History of Sacred and Biblically-Inspired Dance in Australia. Nelen Yubu 6: 3–18.

Kaberry, Phyllis M.
     
1939. Aboriginal Woman, Sacred and Profane. London: Routledge.

Kartomi, Margaret
     
1970. Tjitji Inma at Yalata. Hemisphere 14(6): 33–37.

Keen, Ian
     
1978. One Ceremony, One Song: An Economy of Religious Knowledge among the Yolngu of North-East Arnhem Land. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Australian National University, Canberra.

Kemp, Thérèse B.
     
1968–69. A Propos de Certaines Danses des Aborigénes de la Tasmanie. Ethnographie, Paris 63(3): 156–59.

Kennedy, Peter (ed.)
     
1970. Films on Traditional Music and Dance, a First International Catalogue. Paris: UNESCO for International Folk Music Council.

Laade, Walfgang
     
1968. Etwas über Musik and Tanz bei den Insulanern der Torres-Strasse. Kontakte 4: 121–25.

Lockwood, Douglas W.
     
1963. Crocodiles and Other People. Adelaide: Rigby.

Long, G. MacDonald
     
1937. Corroboree. Walkabout, September: 49, 51, 53, 55.

MacFarlane, Philip H.
     
1950. The Wild-Fowl and the Devil: A Legend of the Torres Strait. Walkabout 16(1): 46–48.

Maddock, Kenneth
     
1972. The Australian Aborigines: A Portrait of Their Society. London: Penguin Books.
     
n.d. Report on Field Work in the Northern Territory 1964–65. Unpublished ms., deposited in AIAS, Canberra.

Major, Thomas

     1900. Leaves from a Squatter's Notebook. London: Sands.

Massola, Aldo
     
1971. The Aborigines of South-Eastern Australia as They Were. Melbourne: Heinemann.

Mathew, John
     
1887. Mary River and Bunya Bunya Country. In Edward M. Curr, The Australian Race. Melboune: Ferres: 3: 125–209.
     
1889. The Australian Aborigines. Royal Society of New South Wales Journal and Proceedings 23: 335–449.
     
1899. Eaglehawk and Crow: A Study of the Australian Aborigines including an Inquiry into their Origin and a Survey of Australian Languages. Melbourne: Melville, Mullen and Slade.

Mathews, Robert H.
     
1898. Initiation Ceremonies of Australian Tribes. American Philosophical Society Proceedings 37: 54–73.

McCarthy, Frederick D.
     
1957. Australia's Aborigines: Their Life and Culture. Melbourne: Colorgravure Publishers.
     
1964. The Dancers of Aurukun. Australian Natural History 14(9): 296-300.
     
1978. Aurukun Dances. Northbridge, NSW. Unpublished ms. deposited in AIAS, Canberra.

McConnel, Ursula H.
     
1930–34. The Wik-munkan Tribe of Cape York Peninsula. Oceania 1(1): 97–194; 1(2): 181–205; 4(3): 310–67.

Meggitt, Mervyn J.
     
1955. Djanba among the Walbiri, Central Australia. Anthropos 50: 375–403.
     
1965 [1962]. Desert People: A Study of the Walbiri Aborigines of Central Australia. Chicago and London: The University of Chicago Press. Originally published by Angus and Robertson, Sydney.
     
1966. Gadjari among the Walbiri of Central Australia. Oceania Monographs no. 14. Sydney: University of Sydney. Originally published in Oceania 36–37.

Morphy Howard
     
1977. "Too Many Meanings:" An Analysis of the Artistic System of the Yongu of North-East Arnhem Land. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis (2 vols.), Australian National University, Canberra.

Morse, Babette
     
1968. Dance Notation and Aboriginal Culture. Hemisphere 12(11): 2–6.

Mountford, Charles P.
     
1956. Expedition to the Land of the Tiwi. National Geographic Magazine 109: 417–40.
     
1958. The Tiwi, Their Art, Myth and Ceremony. London: Phoenix House and Melbourne: Georgian House.
     
1962. Brown Men and Red Sand: Journeying in Wild Australia. Sydney: Angus and Robertson.
     
1976. Nomads of the Australian Desert. Adelaide: Rigby.

Moyle, Alice M.
     
1972. Sound Films for Combined Notation: The Groote Eylandt Field Project, 1969. International Folk Music Council Yearbook 4: 104–18.
     
1977a. Aborigines: Music, Song and Dance. In The Australian Encyclopaedia. 3d ed. Sydney: Grolier Society of Australia: 1: 37–40.
     
1977b. Music and Dance: Mastersingers of the Bush. In The Moving Frontier: Aspects of Aboriginal-European Interaction in Australia (ed. Peter Stanbury). Sydney: Reed.
     
1978. Song and Dance. In Black Australia: An Annotated Bibliography and Teachers' Guide to Resources on Aborigines and Torres Strait Islanders (Comp. M. C. Hill and A. P. C. Barlow). Canberra: AIAS; Atlantic Highlands, NJ: Humanities Press: 63–66.

Moyle, Richard M.
     
1979. Songs of the Pintupi: Musical Life in a Central Australian Society. Canberra: AIAS.

Munn, Nancy D.
     
1973. Walbiri Iconography: Graphic Representations and Cultural Symbolism in a Central Australian Society. Ithaca and London: Cornell University Press.

Peterson, Nicolas
     
1970. Buluwandi: A Central Australian Ceremony for the Resolution of Conflict. In Australian Aboriginal Anthropology (wd. Ronald M. Berndt). Nedlands, Western Australia: University of Western Australia Press for AIAS: 200–15.

Plomley, N. J. B. (ed.)
     
[1966]. Friendly Mission: The Tasmanian Journals and Papers of George Augustus Robinson, 1829–1834. Hobart: Tasmanian Historical Research Association.

Quisenberry, Kay
     
1973. Dance in Arnhem Land: A Field Study Project, 1970–72. Unpublished MFA thesis, Southern Methodist University, Dallas, Texas.

Raven-Hart, Rowland
     
1948. Islands of Torres Strait. Walkabout 14(12): 14–16.
     
1949. The Happy Isles. Melbourne: Georgian House.

Richard, Francis
     
1925. Customs and Language of the Western Hodgkinson Aboriginals. Queensland Museum Memoirs 8(3): 249–65.

Roheim, Geza
     
1933. Women and Their Life in Central Australia. Royal Anthropological Institute Journal 63: 207–65.
     
1943. The Eternal Ones of the Dream. New York: International Universities Press.

Roth, Walter
     
1897. Ethnological Studies among the North-West Central Queensland Aborigines. Brisbane: Government Printer.
     
1902. Games, Spots and Amusements. North Queensland Ethnography: Bulletin no.4. Brisbane: Government Printer.

Salvado, Rosendo
     
1977 [1851]. The Salvado Memoirs: Historical Memoirs of Australia and Particularly of the Benedictine Mission of New Norcia and of the Habits and Customs of the Australian Natives (rrans. and ed. E. J. Storman). Nedlands, Western Australia: University of Western Australia Press. Originally published by Society for the Propagation of the Faith, Rome.

Sansom, Basil
     
1980. The Camp at Wallaby Cross. Canberra: AIAS.

Shannon, Cynthia
     
1971. Walpiri Women's Music. Unpublished BA honours thesis. Monash University, Clayton, Victoria.

Simpson, Colin
     
1971. The Balnooknook Corroboree. In This Land—These People. Sydney: Reader's Digest Association, Australia.

Smyth, Robert B.
     
1878. The Aborigines of Victoria. Melbourne: Government Printer.

Spencer, Sir Walter Baldwin
     
1896. Through Larapinta Land: A Narrative of the Expedition. In Report on the Work of the Horn Scientific Expedition to Central Australia. London: Dulau: 1: 1–136.
     
1914. Native Tribes of the Northern Territory of Australia. London: Macmillan.
     
1928. Wanderings in Wild Australia. 2 vols. London: Macmillan.

Spencer, Sir Walter Baldwin and F. J. Gillen
     
1899. The Native Tribes of Central Australia. London: Macmillan.
     
1904. The Northern Tribes of Central Australia. London: Macmillan.
     
1927. The Arunta: A Study of a Stone Age People. 2 vols. London: Macmillan.

Stanner, William Edward Hanley.
     
1966. On Aboriginal Religion. Oceania Monographs no. 11. Sydney: University of Sydney. Originally published in Oceania 30–34.

Stirling, Edward C.
     
1896. Anthropology. In Report on the Work of the Horn Scientific Expedition . . . [London: Dulau]: 4: 1–157.

Strehlow, Theodor George Henry
     
1971. Songs of Central Australia. Sydney: Angus and Robertson.

Stubington, Jill
     
1977. Songs to Live By. Hemisphere 21(8): 25–30.
     
1979. North Australian Aboriginal Music. In Australian Aboriginal Music (ed. Jennifer Isaacs). Sydney: Aboriginal Artists Agency.

Taplin, George
     
1879. The Folklore, Manners, Customs, and Languages of the South Australian Aborigines. Adelaide: Government Printer.

Thomas, Northcote Whitridge
     
1906. Natives of Australia. London: Archibald Constable & Company.

Thomson, Donald
     
1934.

Notes on a Hero Cult from the Gulf of Carpentaria, North Queensland. Royal Anthropological Institute Journal 64: 217–35.

Thorne, Jessie C.
     
1939. 'Playabout' Corroboree. Wildlife 1(14): 12–13.

Tonkinson, Robert
     
1970. Aboriginal Dream-Spirit Beliefs in a Contact Situation: Jigalong, Western Australia. In Australian Aboriginal Anthropology (ed. Ronald.M. Berndt). Nedlands, Western Australia: University of Western Australia Press for AIAS: 277–91.

Travers, Robert
     
1968. The Tasmanians: The Story of a Doomed Race. Melbourne: Cassell.

Turner, David H.
     
1974. Tradition and Transformation: A Study of the Groote Eylandt Area Aborigines of Northern Australia. Canberra: AIAS.

Von Sturmer, John R.
     
1980. Notes on Dancing at Cannon Hill, Thursday, 18 December 1980: A Demonstration of the Character of Mulil before the Aboriginal Land Commissioner, Mr. Justice Toohey, at the Alligator Rivers Stage 2 Land Claim Hearing. Unpublished ms. deposited in AIAS, Canberra.

Warner, William Lloyd
     
1958 [1937]. A Black Civilization. New York: Harper & Row.

West, Lamont
     
1964. Notes on Tapes in AIAS Archives. Unpublished ms. deposited in AIAS, Canberra.

Wild, Stephen A.
     
1971. Warlbiri Music and Dance in Their Social and Cultural Nexus. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Indiana University, Bloomington.
     
1977. Australian Ritual as Performance: Structure of Communication in Men's Mimetic Dance among Walbiri Aborigines of Central Australia. Paper presented at Conference on Culture and Communication, Temple University, Philadelphia, March 1977. Unpublished ms. deposited at AIAS, Canberra.
     
1977–78. Men as Women: Female Dance Symbolism in Walbiri Men's Rituals. Dance Research Journal 10(1): 14–22. Reprinted in JASHM 4(3) (1986):166–83.
     
1980. Australian Aboriginal Performances, Past and Present. In Souvenir Programme Book, The Festival of Asian Arts, Hong Kong.
     
1981. Aboriginal Music and the Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies. The Australian Journal of Music Education 28: 33–38.

Wolff, H.
     
1938. Nachrichten von der 2 Frobenius—Expedition in Nordwest-Australien. [Communications from the Second Frobenius Expedition in North-West Australia, Collated from the Journals of H. Wolff]. Paideuma bd. 1: 89–99.

Notes:

1For more up-to-date approaches to the subject and work which would be usable by present-day anthropologists and linguists, see Durr (1984), Farnell (1984b), Freedman (1986), Dixon-Stowell (1986), Friedland (1986), Hart-Johnson (1983), Fairbank (1985), Novack (1986), Adra (1986), Puri (1983), Volland (1986), Jablonko (1986), Blakely (1986), Grau (1983) and Dail-Jones (1984).

2Those who for several years worked at and finally succeeded in getting a dance lectureship in Australia were Drs. Stephen Wild, Margaret Clunies-Ross, and Allen Marett.

3For further bibliographic information of a more general anthropological nature, see Burridge (1973: 56–57).

4See Williams (1986) for further discussion.

5Quote marks and a capital C are meant to mark the differences between a technical definition of the term 'culture' in anthropology (which stemmed from Tylor) and a popular misconception of 'Culture,' which tends toward ethnocentric bias, elitism, and connotations of 'superior' and 'inferior' cultural behaviors and artifacts.

6Quisenberry's fieldwork was carried out in Australia, but the thesis was written in the United States and the degree was granted at Southern Methodist University. Although she consulted several Australian anthropologists, their work seemed to have little or no impact on her study seen as a theoretical or methodological work. The anthropologists enter into the discussion only with regard to 'ethnographic' matters. The point is this: Quisenberry's work is a classic example of the notion that theory and description (or ethnography) are somehow separable. Furthermore, her work displays extraordinary epistemological confusions, i.e., she seemed to have no clear idea of the parameters of that which she could comment upon. This could be simply the result of poor supervision; one does not know. Unfortunately, there is only the completed document to consult. Lest I appear to be biased, see Kaeppler (1978: 33), for further criticism of Curt Sachs.

7It is a well-known fact that political and national boundaries do not often coincide well with the linguistic realities of usage among a people; thus, it becomes necessary to stress the point here: Terms of national or political reference rarely do justice to the cultural realities that they attempt to encompass, including, for example, the present mixtures of traditions, even on one Aboriginal reserve where one finds (for example) the Wanam and Apalech traditions co-existing in Aurukun.

8I have tried, using von Sturmer's words, to illustrate exactly what is meant here: This anthropologist's 'description' is an informed description, one which rests not only on intuition and experience of dancing with his informants in the field but on years of scholarly study as well. This is why he can form the sentences in his description the way he does. He understands what the dancing means. His locutions are not simply 'accidents' or the result of a better knowledge of the English language. Should colleagues think that I overstress the relation of theory and description, I would ask that they consult Williams (1986) and then reflect on the value of tutored as against untutored field observations.

9The term 'semasiology' is from a Greek source and can be defined as 'signification' in the sense of 'meaning' + 'logy.' In the late nineteenth century, the word was used to refer to that branch of philology which dealt with the meanings of words. It was used by R. Matineau in 1877 with reference to "the semasiology of Arabic words." In 1884, a reference appeared in the Athenaeum, 27 September 395/1, as follows: "Philology is now advancing towards a new branch having intimate relations with psychology, the so-called semasiology of Abel and others."
     The next recorded use of the term occurs in 1889, where F. Haverfield (Academy 7 Dec. 374/2) uses it to raise doubt about the phonetic connections of words. That is, where two words may seem to be phonetically linked, semasiologically their connection might be improbable. In 1880, a linguistic entity, the 'semasiological solecism' was apparently known and understood, as the phrase occurs in the Athenaeum. In that publication (5 Aug./185) this phrase occurs: "The semasiologist . . . has to trace the vicissitudes which the history of forms, words and phrases presents with respect to signification." Usage of this term has consistently pointed toward the semantic aspects of linguistic signification, and the term is used throughout the texts of my work and that of my students in that sense, only, as it applies to human action sign signification. In anthropology, it is a neologism, and for the reasons why it arose, see Williams (1986).

101 have used a term "semantic primitives" here which is well-known in linguistic literature through the work of Anna Wierzbicka (1980, 1985). I first encountered the phrase not in her books but in Haviland (1986), where he does an extensive analysis of deictic categories, words, and roots with reference to Guugu Ymidihirr (a local Paman language of the Hopevale people) in the southeastern part of the Cape York peninsula. My understanding of the notion of semantic primitives is closer to Haviland's usage than it is to its originator's usage.

11By this, I mean that the only references to theorists of anthropology and the dance that are present in Grau's work are Kaeppler, Keali'inohomoku, and Blacking. This would be all right if we were offered an explanation of the "muted group," i.e., Birdwhistell, Kendon, Williams, Royce, Hall, and others. As it is, the one theoretical framework that includes the notion of universals—worked out in great detail—is ignored: semasiology.

12There is no available space to spell out what is meant here, as the problem would require at least one lecture. Suffice it to say that an anthropologist's relation to his or her informants, to the data, to colleagues, and to the folk-model of events is complex and quite unlike the relations between a so-called hard scientist and his or her work. One starts by teasing out the differences by studying Winch (1958), who stressed these relations and discussed the differences in detail and with great sagacity.

13One is aware of the history of—and current debates over—this term in anthropology. Elsewhere, I have expressed perplexity over Kurath's statement, for example, that "clan totemism produces complex rituals, as in Australia" (1960: 237). There are those who are convinced that 'totemism' in the late 1950s and early 1960s was suffering from its death throes as a major explanatory category in social anthropology, but fuller discussion is impossible here. The term 'totem' is commonly used with regard to dances, and stringent and critical examination needs to be made of such usages, as there is virtually no agreement among writers as to the reality to which the term is supposed to refer.

14I here refer to collections of essays like those of Otten (1971) and Belo (1970), where a good deal of ethnographic fact is elucidated, but these facts are unaccompanied by discussions of what the processes are which generate the facts.

15The ideological basis for the label 'primitive' in connection with Aboriginal peoples has existed in Australia since the mid-eighteenth century at least. It still exists today. Mere mention of the matter is all that is possible here; however, the subject is dealt with in greater detail in Williams (1988), where the notion of 'primitiveness' is connected with a concept of Terra Nullius, which was accompanied by the idea that there were peoples who had not advanced beyond "a state of nature" (see Frost 1981, for more thorough discussion).

References Cited (in addition to those listed in Appendix B):

Adra, Najwa
     
1986. Replicating Culture: Dancing in the Yemen Arab Republic. Paper presented at American Anthropology Association Meetings, Philadelphia.

Ardener, Edwin
     
1975. The Voice of Prophecy: Further Problems in the Analysis of Events. The Munroe Lecture. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University.
     
1982. Social Anthropology, Language and Reality. In Semantic Anthropology (ed. D. Parkin). ASA Volume 22 London: Academic Press: 1–14.

Barish, Jonas
     
1981. The Anti-theatrical Prejudice. Berkeley: University of California Press.

Baxandall, M.
     
1972. Painting and Experience in Fifteenth-Century Italy. London: Oxford University Press.

Bell, Diane
     
1982. Aboriginal Women and the Religious Experience. Bedford Park, SA: Australian Association for the Study of Religion. For the Charles Strong Trust.
     
1983–84. Going It Alone: Practising Applied Anthropology. Anthropological Forum 5(2): 176–81.

Belo, J. (ed.)
     
1970. Traditional Balinese Culture. New York: Columbia University Press.

Berndt, R. M. and E. S. Philips (eds.)
     
1973. The Australian Aboriginal Heritage: An Introduction through the Arts. Sydney: Australian Society for Education through the Arts in association with URE Smith.

Best, David
     
1974. Expression in Movement and the Arts. London: Lepus.
     
1978. Philosophy and Human Movement. New York: Allen & Unwin.
     
1985. Feeling and Reason in the Arts. New York: Allen & Unwin.

Blakeley, Thomas
     1986. Communicative Resources in Hemba Women's Funerary Dance Performance. Paper presented at American Anthropology Association Meetings, Philadelphia.

Burridge, Kenneth
     
1973. Encountering Aborigines: A Case Study, Anthropology, and the Australian Aboriginal. New York: Pergamon Press.

Chapman, Malcolm
     
1982. "Semantics" and "The Celt." In Semantic Anthropology (ed. D. Parkin). ASA Volume 22. London: Academic Press: 123–44.

Clunies-Ross, Margaret, Tamsin Donaldson, and Stephen A. Wild
     
1987. Songs of Aboriginal Australia. Oceanic Monographs 32. Sydney: Oceanic Publications.

Coomeraswamy, Ananda
     
1934. The Transformation of Nature in Art. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
     
1948. The Dance of Shiva: Fourteen Indian Essays. Bombay: Asian Publishing House.
     
1956. Christian and Oriental Philosophy of Art. New York: Dover.

Crick, Malcolm
     
1975. Explorations in Language and Meaning: Towards a Semantic Anthropology. New York: Halstead Press.

Dail-Jones, M. A.
     
1984. A Culture in Motion: A Study of the Interrelationships of Dancing, Sorrowing, Hunting and Fighting as Performed by the Walpiri Women of Central Australia. M.A. thesis, University of Hawaii.

DeMille, Agnes
     
1963. The Book of the Dance. New York: Golden Press.

Dixon-Stowell, Brenda
     
1986. Survival of the Fittest: An Oral History of Selected Aspects of Black Dance in Philadelphia. Paper presented at American Anthropological Association Meetings, Philadelphia.

Durr, Dixie
     
1981. Labanotation: Language or Script? JASHM 1(3): 132–38.
     
1984. The Structure of Ballet Dancing, with Special Emphasis of Roles, Rules, Norms and Statue. Unpublished MA thesis, New York University.

Ellis, Havelock
     1920. Studies in the Psychology of Sex. Philadelphia: F.A. Davis Co.

Fairbank, Holly
     
1985. Chinese Minority Dances: Processors and Preservationists—Part 1. JASHM 3(4): 168–89.
     
1986. Chinese Minority Dances: Processors and Preservationists—Part 2. JASHM 4(1): 1–19.

Farnell, Brenda
     
1984a. Two Sign Languages: A Report on Work in Progress. JASHM 3(1): 8–34.
     
1984b. Visual Communication and Literacy: An Anthropological Enquiry into Plains Indian and American Sign Language. Unpublished MA thesis, New York University.
     
1989. Body Movement Notation. International Encyclopedia of Communications (ed. E. Barnouw). Philadelphia: Oxford University/University of Pennsylvania Press: 203–9.

Fast, Julius
     
1970. Body Language. New York: Evans.

Fillmore, Charles
     
1983. Commentary on Papers of Talmy and Klein. In Spatial Orientation: Theory, Research and Application (ed. H. Pick and L. Acredelo). New York: Plenum Press: 313–20.

Freedman, Diane
     
1986. Dance Code and Gender Role in a Transylvanian Village. Paper presented at American Anthropology Association Meetings, Philadelphia.

Friedland, Lynn
     
1986. Kinesic Genres and Artistic Performance in Afro-American Culture. Paper presented at American Anthropology Association Meetings, Philadelphia.

Frost, A.
     
1981. New South Wales as Terra Nullius: The British Denial of Aboriginal Land Rights Historical Studies 19(7): 513–23.

Goodale, Jane
     
1971. Tiwi Wives: A Study of Women of Melville Island. Seattle: University of Washington Press.

Grau, Andrée
     
1983. Sing a Dance: Dance a Song. Dance Research 1(2): 32–44.

Grove, Lilly
     1895. Dancing. London: Longman, Green.

Hambly, William
     1926. Tribal Dancing and Social Development. London: Witherby.

Hampshire, Stuart
     
1959. Thought and Action. London: Chattus & Windus.

Harrison, Jane
     1948 [1913]. Ancient Art and Ritual. London: Oxford University Press.

Hart-Johnson, Diana
     
1983. On Structure in Martha Graham Technique with Comparisons to Sign Language. JASHM 2(4): 196–210.

Haskell, A.
     
1960. The Story of Dance. London: Rathbone.
     
1969. The Wonderful World of Dance. New York: Doubleday.

Havemeyer, Loomis
     1916. The Drama of Savage Peoples. London: Oxford University Press.

Haviland, John
     
1986. Complex Referential Gestures. (Draft) Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences.

Henson, Hilary
     
1974. British Social Anthropologists and Language: A History of Separate Development. London: Oxford University Press.

Jablonko, Alison
     
1986. Patterns of Daily Life in the Dance of the Maring of New Guinea: From Microanalysis to Macroanalysis. Paper presented at American Anthropology Association Meetings, Philadelphia. Available: Human Studies Film Archive, Smithsonian Institution, Washington D.C.

Jackson. Anthony (ed.)
     
1987. The Anthropologist at Home. ASA Monograph 25. London: Tavistock.

Kaeppler, Adrienne
     
1972. Method and Theory in Analyzing Dance Structure with an Analysis of Tongan Dance. Ethnomusicology 16(2): 173–217.
     
1978. The Dance in Anthropological Perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology 7: 31–39.
     
1985. Structured Movement Systems in Tonga. In Society and the Dance (ed. P. Spencer). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press: 92–118.
     
1986. Cultural Analysis, Linguistic Analogies and the Study of Dance in Anthropological Perspective. In Explorations in Ethnomusicology: Essays in Honor of David P. McAllester (ed. C. Frisbie). Detroit Monographs on Musicology 9. Detroit: Information Coordinates: 25–33.

Keali'inohomoku, Joann
     
1970. Perspective 5: Ethnic Historical Study. CORD Proceedings of the 2d Conference on Research in Dance. New York University, July 4–6, 1969: 86–97.
     
1980 [1969]. An Anthropologist Looks at Ballet as an Ethnic Form of Dance. JASHM 1(2): 83–97. First published in Impulse Magazine 1969.

Kendon, Adam
     
1983. Gesture and Speech: How They Interact. In Non-verbal Interaction (ed. John M. Wiemann and Randall Harrison). Beverley Hills, CA: Sage Publications: 13–45.

Kirstein, Lincoln
     
1924. Dance. New York: F.A. Stokes and Co.

Kurath, Gertrude
     
1960. Panorama of Dance Ethnology. Current Anthropology 1(3): 233–54.

Lange, Roderyk
     1975. The Nature of Dance: An Anthropological View. London: MacDonald and Evens.

Levi-Strauss, Claude
     
1966. La Pensée Sauvage [The Savage Mind]. London: Weidensfeld & Nicholson

Martin, John
     
1939. Introduction to the Dance. New York: Norton.
     
1963. Book of the Dance. New York: Tudor. First Published as The Dance 1947.

McNeill, David
     
1979. The Conceptual Basis of Language. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.

McNeill, David and E. Levy
     
1982. Conceptual Representation in Language Activity and Gesture. In Speech Place and Action (ed. R. Jarvella and P. Klein). Chichester, UK: Wiley: 271–95.

Morris, Desmond, Peter Collett, Peter Marsh, and Marie O'Shaughnessy
     
1979. Gestures: Their Origins and Distributions. London: Cape.

Moyle, Richard
     
1984. Jumping to Conclusions. In Problems and Solutions: Occasional Essays in Musicology. Presented to Alice M. Moyle (ed. J. Kassler and J. Stubington). Sydney: Hale and Iremonger: 51–58.

Novack, Cynthia
     
1986. Dance as Social Text. Paper presented at American Anthropology Association Meetings, Philadelphia.

Otten C. (ed.)
     
1971. Anthropology and Art: Readings in Cross-Cultural Aesthetics. New York: Natural History Press.

Pocock, David
     
1967. The Anthropology of Time-Reckoning. In Myth and Cosmos (ed. J Middleton). New York: Natural History Press: 303–14.

Pouwer, Jan
     
1973. Signification and Fieldwork. Journal of Symbolic Anthropology 1: 1–13.

Puri, Rakika
     
1983. A Structural Analysis of Meaning in Movement: The Hand Gestures of Indian Classical Dance. Unpublished MA thesis, New York University.

Ridgeway, William
     1915. Dramas and Dramatic Dances of Non-European Peoples. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Sachs, Curt
     
1937. World History of the Dance (trans. Bessie Schoenberg). London: Allen & Unwin.

Sheets, Maxine
     
1966. The Phenomenology of Dance. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.

Sorell, Walter
     
1967. Dance through the Ages. New York: Grosset & Dunlap.

Spencer, Paul (ed.)
     
1985. Society and the Dance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Stanner, W. E. H.
     
1979. The White Man Got No Dreaming: Essays 1938–1973. Canberra: Australian National University Press.

Terry, Walter
     
1956. The Dance in America. New York: Harper Bros.
     
1967. Dance, History of. In The Dance Encyclopedia (ed. A. Chujoy and P. Manchester). New York: Simon & Shuster: 234–43.

Tylor, E. B.
     
1878. The Early History of Mankind. 3d ed. London: J Murray; and New York: Rinehart & Winston.

Volland, A.
     
1986. Movement and Metaphor in a South Indian Dance-form. Paper presented at American Anthropology Association Meetings, Philadelphia.

Von Sturmer, John
     
1987. Aboriginal Singing and Notions of Power. In Songs of Aboriginal Australia (ed. Clunies-Ross et al.): 63–76
     
1998. Aborigines of Cape York. In Australian Aboriginal Dance. The International Encyclopedia of Dance. Selma Jean Cohen and Dance Perspectives Foundation. © 1998, 2005 by Oxford University Press, Inc. The International Encyclopedia of Dance: (e-reference edition). Oxford University Press. University of Illinois-Urbana Champaign. http://www.oxford-dance.com/entry?entry=t171.e0101.s0004 (June 25, 2012).

Wierzbicka, Anna
     
1980. Lingua Mentalis: The Semantics of Natural Language. Sydney: Academic Press
     
1985. Lexicography and Conceptual Analysis. Ann Arbor, MI: Karoma

Wild, Stephen A., Margaret Clunies-Ross, Andrée Grau, John Von Sturmer, Megan Dail-Jones, and Catherine J. Ellis
     
1998. Australian Aboriginal Dance. In The International Encyclopedia of Dance. Selma Jean Cohen and Dance Perspectives Foundation. © 1998, 2005 by Oxford University Press, Inc. The International Encyclopedia of Dance: (e-reference edition). Oxford University Press. University of Illinois-Urbana Champaign. http://www.oxford-dance.com/entry?entry=t171.e0101.s0001 (June 25, 2012).

Williams Drid
     
1975. The Role of Movement in Selected Symbolic Systems. Unpublished D.Phil. thesis, Oxford University.
     
1976a. An Exercise in Applied Personal Anthropology. Dance Research Journal (CORD) 9(1): 16–30.
     
1976b. Deep Structures of the Dance. Part 1: Constitutent Syntagmatic Analysis. Journal of Human Movement Studies (ed. J. Whiting) 2(2): 123–44.
     
1976c. Deep Structures of the Dance. Part 2: The Conceptual Space of the Dance. Journal of Human Movement Studies (ed. J. Whiting) 2(3): 155–71.
     
1976d. Review of R. Lange, The Nature of Dance: An Anthropological Perspective. Dance Research Journal (CORD) 9(1): 42–44.
     
1980a. Taxonomies of the Body, with Special Reference to the Ballet, Part 1. JASHM 1(1): 1–19.
     
1980b. Taxonomies of the Body, with Special Reference to the Ballet, Part 2. JASHM 1(2): 98–122.
     
1980c. On Structures of Human Movement: A Reply to Gell. JASHM 6(4): 303–22.
     
1982. Semasiology: A Semantic Anthropologists View of Human Movements and Actions. In Semantic Anthropology (ed. D. Parkin). ASA 22. London: Academic Press: 168–82.
     
1986. Prefigurements of Art: A Reply to Sebeok. JASHM 4(2): 68–80.
     
1988. Homo Nullius: The Status of Traditional Dancing in Northern Queensland. Paper presented at International Conference on Hunters and Gatherers (Anthropology) Darwin, NT, August 31.
     
1991. Ten Lectures on Theories of the Dance. Metuchen, NJ: Scarecrow Press.
     
2004 [1991]. Anthropology of the Dance: Ten Lectures. 2d ed. Champaign: University of Illinois Press. Updated and revised Ten Lectures 1991.

Winch, Peter
     
1958. The Idea of a Social Science and Its Relation to Philosophy. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

Youngerman, Suzanne
     
1974. Curt Sachs and His Heritage: A Critical Review of World History of the Dance with a Survey of Recent Studies That Perpetuate his Ideas. CORD News 6(2): 6–19.

 

 
 
 
 

Content in Journal for the Anthropological Study of Human Movement (ISSN 1940-7610) is intended for personal, noncommercial use only. You may not reproduce, publish, distribute, transmit, participate in the transfer or sale of, modify, create derivative works from, display, or in any way exploit the Journal for the Anthropological Study of Human Movement database in whole or in part without the written permission of the copyright holder.
 
 
© 2012 by the Board of Trustees of the University of Illinois
Terms and Conditions of Use.